Our systems are now restored following recent technical disruption, and we’re working hard to catch up on publishing. We apologise for the inconvenience caused. Find out more: https://www.cambridge.org/universitypress/about-us/news-and-blogs/cambridge-university-press-publishing-update-following-technical-disruption

We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings .

Login Alert

  • > Journals
  • > Australian Journal of Environmental Education
  • > Volume 21
  • > The Role of Social Research in Effective Social Change...

how can a research project cause social change

Article contents

The role of social research in effective social change programs.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 June 2015

Social research is a critical foundation for programs that seek to engage communities in change and in the development of more sustainable societies. Without appropriate research, programs aimed at change are likely to be based on implicit or assumed problem identification and/or inferred community needs and wishes. If we are to achieve community participation in activities that lead to real change, research to find out about those communities is the first step. Over the past ten years the NSW Department of Environment and Conservation (DEC) has developed a considerable body of social research, conducted with both the general community and specific community segments, to underpin its environmental education programs. This paper includes a review of some models for integrating social research into education programs, and examines the extent to which social research has impacted on both the environmental education programs and the organisational culture of the DEC. From this are drawn learnings from the perspective of a major State environmental agency, about the integration of social research into any program or organisation seeking to achieve social change towards sustainability. As well as program specific benefits, the ultimate outcome of this process is to assist in producing an organisational culture which values evidence-based decision-making and develops policies and structures that incorporate a social research dimension into both programs and policy.

Crossref logo

This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by Crossref .

  • Google Scholar

View all Google Scholar citations for this article.

Save article to Kindle

To save this article to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle .

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Lynne McLoughlin (a1) and Geoff Young (a1)
  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0814062600000951

Save article to Dropbox

To save this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Dropbox account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox .

Save article to Google Drive

To save this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Google Drive account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive .

Reply to: Submit a response

- No HTML tags allowed - Web page URLs will display as text only - Lines and paragraphs break automatically - Attachments, images or tables are not permitted

Your details

Your email address will be used in order to notify you when your comment has been reviewed by the moderator and in case the author(s) of the article or the moderator need to contact you directly.

You have entered the maximum number of contributors

Conflicting interests.

Please list any fees and grants from, employment by, consultancy for, shared ownership in or any close relationship with, at any time over the preceding 36 months, any organisation whose interests may be affected by the publication of the response. Please also list any non-financial associations or interests (personal, professional, political, institutional, religious or other) that a reasonable reader would want to know about in relation to the submitted work. This pertains to all the authors of the piece, their spouses or partners.

Encyclopedia Britannica

  • History & Society
  • Science & Tech
  • Biographies
  • Animals & Nature
  • Geography & Travel
  • Arts & Culture
  • Games & Quizzes
  • On This Day
  • One Good Fact
  • New Articles
  • Lifestyles & Social Issues
  • Philosophy & Religion
  • Politics, Law & Government
  • World History
  • Health & Medicine
  • Browse Biographies
  • Birds, Reptiles & Other Vertebrates
  • Bugs, Mollusks & Other Invertebrates
  • Environment
  • Fossils & Geologic Time
  • Entertainment & Pop Culture
  • Sports & Recreation
  • Visual Arts
  • Demystified
  • Image Galleries
  • Infographics
  • Top Questions
  • Britannica Kids
  • Saving Earth
  • Space Next 50
  • Student Center
  • Introduction
  • Historical background
  • Cyclic change
  • One-directional change
  • Combined patterns of change

Natural environment

Demographic processes, technological innovations, economic processes, social movements, political processes.

  • Mechanisms of one-directional change: accumulation, selection, and differentiation
  • Mechanisms of curvilinear and cyclic change: saturation and exhaustion
  • Conflict, competition, and cooperation
  • Tension and adaptation
  • Diffusion of innovations
  • Planning and institutionalization of change

Karl Marx

Explanations of social change

Our editors will review what you’ve submitted and determine whether to revise the article.

  • BCcampus Open Publishing - Introduction to Sociology – 1st Canadian Edition - Social Movements and Social Change
  • Social Sciences LibreTexts Library - Understanding Social Change
  • Roger Williams University Open Publishing - Social Change
  • Frontiers - Toward a Psychology of Social Change: A Typology of Social Change
  • University of Minnesota Libraries - Open Textbooks - Understanding Social Change
  • Table Of Contents

One way of explaining social change is to show causal connections between two or more processes. This may take the form of determinism or reductionism , both of which tend to explain social change by reducing it to one supposed autonomous and all-determining causal process. A more cautious assumption is that one process has relative causal priority, without implying that this process is completely autonomous and all-determining. What follows are some of the processes thought to contribute to social change.

Changes in the natural environment may result from climatic variations, natural disasters, or the spread of disease. For example, both the worsening of climatic conditions and the Black Death epidemics are thought to have contributed to the crisis of feudalism in 14th-century Europe. Changes in the natural environment may be either independent of human activities or caused by them. Deforestation, erosion, air pollution , and contemporary climate change belong to the latter category, and they in turn may have far-reaching social consequences.

Population growth and increasing population density represent demographic forms of social change. Population growth may lead to geographic expansion of a society, military conflicts, and the intermingling of cultures . Increasing population density may stimulate technological innovations , which in turn may increase the division of labour , social differentiation, commercialization, and urbanization . This sort of process occurred in western Europe from the 11th to the 13th century and in England in the 18th century, where population growth spurred the Industrial Revolution . On the other hand, population growth may contribute to economic stagnation and increasing poverty , as may be witnessed in several developing countries today.

Several theories of social evolution identify technological innovations as the most important determinants of societal change. Such technological breakthroughs as the smelting of iron, the introduction of the plow in agriculture, the invention of the steam engine , and the development of computers and the Internet have had lasting social consequences.

Technological changes are often considered in conjunction with economic processes. These include the formation and extension of markets, modifications of property relations (such as the change from feudal lord-peasant relations to contractual proprietor-tenant relations), and changes in the organization of labour (such as the change from independent craftsmen to factories). Historical materialism , as developed by Marx and Engels, is one of the more prominent theories that gives priority to economic processes, but it is not the only one. Indeed, materialist theories have even been developed in opposition to Marxism. One of these theories, the “ logic of industrialization” thesis by the American scholar Clark Kerr and his colleagues, states that industrialization everywhere, including in the mid-20th-century communist countries, has similar consequences.

Other theories have stressed the significance of ideas as causes of social change. Comte’s law of three stages is such a theory. Weber regarded religious ideas as important contributors to economic development or stagnation; according to his controversial thesis, the individualistic ethic of Christianity, and in particular Calvinism , partially explains the rise of the capitalist spirit, which led to economic dynamism in the West.

A change in collective ideas is not merely an intellectual process; it is often connected to the formation of new social movements . This in itself might be regarded as a potential cause of social change. Weber called attention to this factor in conjunction with his concept of “ charismatic leadership .” Charismatic leaders, by virtue of the extraordinary personal qualities attributed to them, are able to create a group of followers who are willing to break established rules. Examples include Jesus, Napoleon, and Hitler. In later social theory, however, the concept of charisma was trivialized to refer to almost any popular figure.

Changes in the regulation of violence, in the organization of the state, and in international relations may also contribute to social change. For example, German sociologist Norbert Elias interpreted the formation of states in western Europe as a relatively autonomous process that led to increasing control of violence and, ultimately, to rising standards of self-control. According to other theories of political revolution, such as those proposed by the American historical sociologist Charles Tilly, the functioning of the state apparatus itself and the nature of interstate relations are of decisive importance in the outbreak of a revolution : it is only when the state is not able to fulfill its basic functions of maintaining law and order and defending territorial integrity that revolutionary groups have any chance of success.

Each of these processes may contribute to others; none is the sole determinant of social change. One reason why deterministic or reductionist theories are often disproved is that the method for explaining the processes is not autonomous but must itself be explained. Moreover, social processes are often so intertwined that it would be misleading to consider them separately. For example, there are no fixed borders between economic and political processes, nor are there fixed boundaries between economic and technological processes. Technological change may in itself be regarded as a specific type of organizational or conceptual change. The causal connections between distinguishable social processes are a matter of degree and vary over time.

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Toward a psychology of social change: a typology of social change.

\r\nRoxane de la Sablonnire*

  • Social Change and Identity Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada

Millions of people worldwide are affected by dramatic social change (DSC). While sociological theory aims to understand its precipitants, the psychological consequences remain poorly understood. A large-scale literature review pointed to the desperate need for a typology of social change that might guide theory and research toward a better understanding of the psychology of social change. Over 5,000 abstracts from peer-reviewed articles were assessed from sociological and psychological publications. Based on stringent inclusion criteria, a final 325 articles were used to construct a novel, multi-level typology designed to conceptualize and categorize social change in terms of its psychological threat to psychological well-being. The typology of social change includes four social contexts: Stability, Inertia, Incremental Social Change and, finally, DSC. Four characteristics of DSC were further identified: the pace of social change, rupture to the social structure, rupture to the normative structure, and the level of threat to one's cultural identity. A theoretical model that links the characteristics of social change together and with the social contexts is also suggested. The typology of social change as well as our theoretical proposition may serve as a foundation for future investigations and increase our understanding of the psychologically adaptive mechanisms used in the wake of DSC.

“Change— extremely rapid social change —is the most important fact of life today”

( Nolan and Lenski, 2011 , p. xiii).

Zoia is a lively 75-year-old Baboushka . Her eventful life has seen her experience some less-than-welcome adventures, but she has always managed to adapt to unfamiliar circumstances. After completing her studies in Moscow, she was, like many other young educated Russians, deported by USSR authorities to another state. Her destination was Frunze (later renamed Bishkek), a land in Central Asia warmer than hers and made slightly cooler by its unfamiliarity. Despite the diversity of Frunze, with ethnic Kyrgyz, Ukrainians, and other Slavic groups forming sizeable minorities, the Russian population remained a majority. During the Soviet era, Zoia was told that she lived in one of the most powerful countries in the world, where crime rates were low and the population enjoyed decent education and food supply, as well as the opportunity to save money for retirement.

The diversity of ethnicities eventually bred great tension, and the collapse of the Soviet Union in the early 1990s deeply affected Zoia's life. At the age of 54, she learned that her country was in ruins, that her rights as a Russian were diminished and that her language was widely frowned upon within the newly formed Kyrgyz Republic, Kyrgyzstan. Meanwhile, the disorganized authority allowed for an explosion in crime rates and increasing scarcity of resources. Zoia lost all of her life savings. The money she earned was no longer sufficient to cover basic necessities. Despite her position as a chief engineer, Zoia was forced to work a second job selling newspapers at the corner of her street just to make ends meet.

Although Zoia's story may seem uniquely dramatic, it is only one among over one billion ( Sun and Ryder, 2016 ). Social change is indiscriminately pervasive and global—restricted to neither developing nor western worlds (e.g., Ponsioen, 1962 ; Smith, 1973 ; Chirot and Merton, 1986 ; Zuck, 1997 ; Sztompka, 1998 ; Fukuyama, 1999 ; Weinstein, 2010 ; Nolan and Lenski, 2011 ; Greenfield, 2016 ). Dramatic social change (DSC) is the new normal and can be witnessed presently across a multitude of contexts from political and economic upheaval, to desperate mass migration, and from natural or human disasters to technological advances.

Social change has always been a field of great interest for the social sciences, especially among sociologists since it seems that “all sociology is about change” ( Sztompka, 1993 , p.xiii; see also Sztompka, 2004 ). Many sociology texts have entire sections devoted to social change (e.g., Bauman, 2003 ; Latour, 2005 ; Hewitt et al., 2008 ; Giddens et al., 2011 ) all aimed at addressing one main question: What leads to social change ? Many sociological theories have been suggested to explain the different “macro” processes associated with the onset of revolutions, social movements, or important technological changes. A “macro” theory focuses on the structural factors or defining events that contribute to DSC and are useful when considering how social changes are brought upon an entire group, community, institution, nation, or indeed society as a whole. The macro approach, however, is seriously limited when it comes to “micro” processes, which focus on the equally important question of the consequences of social change, or, in other words, how individual group members are impacted by social change (e.g., Rogers, 2003 ). Thus, the exclusive research focus on macro processes has left unanswered the pivotal question: What are the psychological consequences of social change?

Given the potentially dire consequences of DSC, it is surprising that psychologists have neglected it as a topic of rigorous academic pursuit, particularly given the current reality of vast globalization and massive immigration. To date, research focusing on the impact of social change on the well-being of individuals has not been clearly established ( Kim, 2008 ; Liu et al., 2014 ). Moreover, the adaptation mechanisms that people develop when coping with such contexts remain largely unknown ( Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004 ).

The goal of the present paper is to argue that psychology needs to focus on the psychology of social change ( de la Sablonnière et al., 2013 ; de la Sablonnière and Usborne, 2014 ). I argue that the bridge between the “macro” processes of social change and the “micro” processes of its psychological impacts have yet to be built. I suggest that social scientists must first focus on conceptualizing social change in a manner that includes both macro and micro processes in order to understand individuals' adaptation to social change. Thus, as the first step in moving toward a psychology of social change, I target what is considered the most difficult challenge: conceptualizing social change.

First and foremost, conceptualizing social change requires untangling the complexity of the topic by formulating a typology of social change (see Table 1 ). To that end, a large-scale meta-review that assembled original perspectives, theories and definitions of social change within both the sociological and psychological literature was performed. The typology of social change that emerged distinguishes four separate social contexts associated with social change: stability, inertia, incremental social change, and DSC. DSC, because of its frequency in today's world, and because it is threatening to people, requires special attention. Thus, the proposed typology of social change drills deeper and articulates four necessary characteristics for a change or an event to be labeled as “dramatic social change”: rapid pace of change, rupture in social structure, rupture in normative structure, and threat to cultural identity. Finally, I come full circle by proposing a theoretical model that links together the four characteristics of DSC within the proposed typology of social change (see Figure 1 ). In sum, the typology of social change I am suggesting can be useful to create a theoretical consensus among researchers about what social change is that perhaps will allow for a coordinated, evidence based strategy to address the psychology of social change.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The typology of social change .

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Proposed theoretical model .

Social Change in Sociology and Psychology

Today, the field of sociology is at the forefront of social change theory and research, with a particular focus upon the factors that constitute and are prerequisites to social change. Within the sociological literature, three main theories have been championed for their attempt to explain social change: Evolutionary Theory, Conflict Theory , and Functionalist Theory . Each theory is characterized by key descriptive interpretations in Table 2 where a global overview of the conceptualization of social change is offered 1 .

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Theories of social change in sociology .

Despite the first appearance of “social change” in the psychological literature more than 70 years ago, only a few isolated psychologists have focused on social change per se and even fewer have offered a clear definition or conceptualization of the concept. The first paper that defined social change was published in the Academy of Political and Social Science and was entitled Psychology of Social Change . Social change was defined as “always a slow and gradual process” ( Marquis, 1947 , p. 75). From that point in time to the dissolution of the Soviet Union in 1991, there have been very few attempts to reintroduce social change into the field of psychology (e.g., Pizer and Travers, 1975 ; Schneiderman, 1988 ). However, after the dissolution of the Soviet Union and the fall of the Berlin Wall, there has been a small surge of research on social change in psychology. For example, several edited books (e.g., Thomas and Veno, 1992 ; Breakwell and Lyons, 1996 ; Crockett and Silbereisen, 2000 ) and special issues of journals ( Silbereisen and Tomasik, 2010 ; Blackwood et al., 2013 ) have focused exclusively on social change and on people's reactions to it. For clarity purposes, Table 3 attempts to summarize the various theories or perspectives in different subfields of cultural and social psychology while Table 4 attempts to do so in subfields of psychology.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Theories and perspectives addressing social change in social psychology .

www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Theories addressing social change in subfields of psychology .

Limitations of Current Research and Conceptualization of Social Change in Sociology and Psychology

As indicated in the summary tables, both contemporary and traditional theorists in sociology and psychology have addressed social change through a variety of macro sociological or societal lenses, and equally from a plethora of micro, psychological, or individual perspectives. Theory and research thus far has demonstrated that social change is a complex entity (e.g., McGrath, 1983 ; Buchanan et al., 2005 ; Subašić et al., 2012 ) that can be conceptualized in many diverging (and confusing) ways. The challenge associated with defining social change may well be to explain why it is an understudied phenomenon ( de la Sablonnière et al., 2013 ) and highlight the challenge of moving forward in studying its psychological impact on ordinary people. The typology of social change presented here offers an initial attempt at clarifying the meaning of social change from a psychological perspective. That is, I focus on an individualistic perspective, but attempt to address the role that macro processes play in terms of our more micro or psychological focus. Here, I discuss three main issues that point to the necessity to properly conceptualize DSC.

First, and most importantly, the conceptualization and understanding of social change does not reach a consensus within the scientific literature (e.g., Coughlin and Khinduka, 1976 ). Furthermore, few scientists define precisely what they mean when using the concept (e.g., Saran, 1963 ). For example, when social change is studied from a social identity theory perspective ( Tajfel and Turner, 1986 ), or a sociological conflict theory perspective, social change is conceptualized almost exclusively in the context of collective action ( Krznaric, 2007 ). In light of this, collective action is defined as a means for group members to achieve an improved social position for their group in the social hierarchy ( Taylor and McKirnan, 1984 ; Batel and Castro, 2015 ; de Lemus and Stroebe, 2015 ). In contrast, cultural psychology and developmental psychology conceptualize social change in a broader manner (e.g., societal transformations such as the fall of the Soviet Union; immigration) where change is not limited to the context of intergroup conflict ( Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004 ; Sun and Ryder, 2016 ). The fact that there is divergence in conceptualizing social change is preventing coordinated research on social change, because not all types of social change are considered. With some theories (e.g., relative deprivation theory, social identity theory, evolutionary theory, conflict theory), social change is conceived mostly as an autonomously controlled and unidirectional process toward group change; these conceptualizations do not account for social changes that are outside of human control, such as natural disasters (e.g., Coughlin and Khinduka, 1976 ). Equating social change with collective action (see Stroebe et al., 2015 ), for example, neglects uncontrollable social transformations such as socio-political reforms and natural disasters over which individuals or groups exert no control. Indeed, the majority of individuals who experience DSC have little control over such events. Since previous classifications can only explain some instances of social change, a theory that would clarify the characteristics required in conceptualizing DSC for all types of change has become a necessity.

The second issue that points to the need for a typology of social change is that not all social contexts associated with social change (i.e., stability and inertia) were considered in previous scientific literature. Most theoretical and empirical work on social change in both sociology and psychology has focused on either incremental social change or DSC (e.g., Andersson et al., 2014 ; Bernstrøm and Kjekshus, 2015 ). However, in order to have a complete theory or typology of social change, it is also necessary to take into account social contexts where there is no social change, contexts of either stability or inertia (Table 1 ). Knowing about incremental social change, inertia and stability, as well as how they relate to DSC is psychologically critical. A clear definition of the four social contexts of social change can facilitate finding solutions for the population to not only the consequences associated with DSC, but also the considerable and potentially unique challenges associated with each of these social contexts (see Abrams and Vasiljevic, 2014 ). For example, a society in a state of inertia may be misconceived as a society in a state of DSC if no clear understanding of each social context is achieved. In inertia, there might be less hope for reverting to a healthy society and consequently less long-term goals that are developed, whereas a time of DSC, such as a political revolution, may provide some hope for the future and some possibilities for some concrete long-term goals. Although the main focus of our paper is DSC, the full spectrum of social contexts associated with social change is presented. A more comprehensive theory of social change capable of accounting for stability, inertia as well as incremental and DSC is required to fully understand the psychological processes and ramifications of social change. Moreover, it is important to define stability, inertia, and incremental social change because they serve as a base for comparison or contrast to DSC. As Calhoun notes: “To understand social change, thus, it is necessary also to understand what produces social continuity” ( Calhoun, 2000 , p. 2642).

Finally, the third issue that pushes me to develop a typology of social change is that, mainly in sociology, a specific event that can be characterized as social change can be interpreted in light of different theories of social change. Let us take the 2005 Tulip Revolution in Kyrgyzstan as an example. Evolutionary theorists may argue that this revolution followed the natural evolution of Kyrgyz society. On the other hand, functionalist theorists may argue that there was disequilibrium in Kyrgyzstan at the time of the revolution. However, it would be beneficial to conceptualize social change the same way in order to be able to assess its impact on individuals. What is needed is a conceptualization of social change that can be interpreted in light of all the theories and processes that have been developed thus far. When an in-depth analysis of the literature is performed, the essential characteristics that define social change across theories may be ascertained. For example, one of the characteristics that was identified in conceptualizing DSC was the rapid pace of social change. The rapid vs. slow pace of social change is important, for instance, to distinguish a DSC from an incremental social change where transformations in the social structure take place without major disruptions. Whether one conceptualizes social change from a functionalist theory, a social identity theory, or a developmental theory perspective, most researchers from these distinctive fields point to the pace of change as one pivotal and essential element that characterizes DSC. Thus, when I base the typology of social change upon such characteristics, garnered from previous research in both sociology and psychology, an all-encompassing conceptualization of social change may be obtained, and later used to guide empirical research independently of the diverging theoretical perspectives.

My observations on the limitations of sociology and psychology should not detract from the insightful contributions these disciplines have made to our understanding of social change. Indeed, these social scientists have tapped into very important issues. For example, although collective action is not the only type of social change, the research on this topic has successfully identified factors that lead individuals and groups to be dissatisfied with their conditions and engage in collective action. However, as Sampson (1989) pointed out: “we have not gone far enough in connecting our theories of the person with social change, in particular, with major historic transformation in the social world” (p. 417). Since our contemporary social world is characterized by social change ( Weinstein, 2010 ), like Sampson (1989) , I argue that “a psychology for tomorrow is a psychology that begins actively to chart out a theory of the person that is no longer rooted in the liberal individualistic assumptions, but is reframed in terms more suitable to resolving the issues of a global era” (p. 431).

In sum, social change needs to be clearly examined because future research is limited without an all-inclusive typology of social change; one that can bridge the epistemological differences between theories from various fields of research and diverging theoretical perspectives. What is needed is a clear conceptualization of social change that considers, and includes, the different characteristics that compose DSC and that were suggested by researchers from all these diverging areas and theoretical orientations.

Constructing a Typology of Social Change: The Characteristics of DSC

Two separate databases from sociology and psychology were targeted to collate relevant peer-reviewed publications: Sociology Abstracts and PsycInfo. Including the year 2016, a total of 5,676 abstracts were carefully analyzed (90% inter-judge reliability; Table 5 ). Two inclusion criteria were used to determine if a manuscript was relevant to our typology of social change. First, the selected abstract, and then the articles, needed to a) focus on social change by including a relevant original definition or providing an original perspective on the concept (originality), or b) focus on one's perspective of social change at either the individual or group level (perceptions).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Number of abstracts and articles that satisfied the specified inclusion criteria .

When reviewing the literature, I had one main goal: selecting and identifying the necessary characteristics of DSC that could either be present or not in other social contexts (i.e., stability, inertia, and incremental social change). Scientists refer to the characteristics in two different ways: (1) formally, when defining or describing DSC, incremental social change, stability, or inertia, and (2) informally, when introducing their research on social change 2 . I made sure that the included articles sufficiently addressed one or more of the four selected characteristics (i.e., rapid pace of change, rupture in social structure, rupture in normative structure, and threat to cultural identity, see Table 6 ). These four characteristics were chosen after a first reading of each of the articles (up to October 2013). They emerged most consistently and were singled out more often for their importance. From prior knowledge, I anticipated that “pace of change” and “social structure” would surface. The other two emerged naturally. From prior knowledge, I also expected the term “valence of change” (i.e., negative change) to emerge (e.g., Slone et al., 2002 ; de la Sablonnière and Tougas, 2008 ; de la Sablonnière et al., 2009c ; Kim, 2008 ). However, that characteristic did not appear in a significant number of papers. The fact that some authors report “positive” change as having negative consequences (e.g., Prislin and Christensen, 2005 ; Bruscella, 2015 ) and “negative” change as having positive consequence (e.g., Yakushko, 2008 ; Abrams and Vasiljevic, 2014 ) may explain why the valence did not emerge as an important characteristic of DSC.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Characteristics of dramatic social change .

To conceptualize an event as DSC, all four characteristics must be present. For example, if an event is affecting only the normative structure in a gradual manner, it would not be possible to label that event as DSC. As for the other three social contexts (stability, inertia, and incremental social change), each has its own unique configuration of characteristics (see Figure 1 ) 3 .

The Pace of Change

The first characteristic that emerged regards the pace , which could either be slow or rapid, and is defined as the speed at which an event impacts a collectivity . When defining social change, researchers from both sociology and psychology distinguish two types of social change based on the pace of change: incremental (e.g., first-order change, beta change, decline, gradual, small-scale) and dramatic (e.g., second-order, gamma, abrupt, collapse, large-scale).

Theories of social change have explicitly and/or implicitly acknowledged the pace of social change as a central determining factor toward its characterization. For example, in one of the earliest versions of their seminal book, Lenski and Lenski (1974) state: “The most striking feature of contemporary life is the revolutionary pace of social change. Never before have things changed so fast for so much of mankind” ( Lenski and Lenski, 1974 , p. 3, see also Fried, 1964 ; Rudel and Hooper, 2005 ). In their new edition entitled Human Societies: An Introduction to Macrosociology , Nolan and Lenski (2011) describe how slowly human evolution has progressed for thousands of years until about 100 years ago, when humans began to evolve at an accelerated pace. Similarly, Weinstein (2010) suggests that for the last few decades, there has been “rapid and accelerating rates of change in human relations, from the interpersonal to the international level” (p. xvii).

It is worthwhile to note that a few key authors refer to pace when distinguishing different types of social change. For example, in organizational psychology, Nadler and Tushman (1995) distinguish slow “incremental” change from fast “discontinuous” change, where the latter would be characterized as DSC in the typology of social change. According to these authors, incremental changes are intended to continually improve the fit among the components of an organization. These changes can either be small or large; nonetheless, there is a succession of manageable changes and adaptation processes. In contrast, discontinuous changes are often linked to major changes in the global scope of the industry and involve a complete break with the past as well as a major reconstruction of almost all elements of the organization. These changes are more traumatic, painful, and demanding as individuals are required to acquire a whole new set of behaviors and discard old patterns. These dramatic changes are not made to improve the fit, but to construct a new collectivity, be it a nation-state, institution or sub-group of the larger collectivity. Newman (2000) also distinguishes between first-order change and second-order change in the context of organizations. According to him, a first-order change, which is equivalent to incremental social change, “is most likely during times of relative environmental stability and is likely to take place over extended periods of time” ( Newman, 2000 , p.604). In other words, this type of change occurs slowly and allows the organization and its members to adapt to the changes gradually. However, a second-order change, or DSC, is radical, and transforms the core of the organization ( Newman, 2000 ). In this case, the change is so sudden that it does not necessarily allow individuals to adapt to the process ( Buchanan et al., 2005 ). Similarly, Rogers (2003) defines social change as abrupt and arises when the entire system is modified and jeopardized because changes are too fast for the system to adjust. In his book, Diamond (2005) contrasts “decline”—where minor ups and downs do not restructure the society—with “collapse”—an extreme form of several milder types of decline—which make it a DSC. An example of collapse is when most of the inhabitants of a population vanish as a result of ecological disasters, starvation, war, or disease. Examples of this are genocides such as Rwanda's which claimed around 800,000 lives, destroyed much of the country's infrastructure and displaced four million people ( Des Forges, 1999 ; Zorbas, 2004 ; Pham et al., 2004 ; Staub et al., 2005 ; Schaal and Elbert, 2006 ; Prunier, 2010 ; Yanagizawa-Drott, 2014 ), the Armenian Massacres, which saw the systematic extermination of about 1.5 million minority Armenians in Turkey ( Dadrian, 1989 , 1998 ) or Cambodia's genocide, which involved the death of almost two million people through the Khmer Rouge's policies of relocation, mass executions, torture, forced labor, malnutrition, and disease ( Hannum, 1989 ). All these events led to an inordinate number of deaths and population movements in a short, restricted period of time.

To be considered dramatic , a social change needs to be quick and must involve a “break with the past” ( Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ; see also Armenakis et al., 1986 ). The example most often used in the literature is the breakdown of the communist system in Eastern Europe and the Soviet Union (e.g., Kollontai, 1999 ; Pinquart et al., 2009 ; Round and Williams, 2010 ; Walker and Stephenson, 2010 ; Chen, 2015 ). For example, when Pinquart et al. (2004 , p. 341) introduced their research on social change, they made a distinction between “gradual” change, such as ideological change in many Western societies, and “abrupt social change,” which represents a form of social change that may be spurred by a sudden, dramatic transformation of economic, political, and social institutions.

Rupture in the Social Structure

The second characteristic of DSC that emerges from my review regards a rupture in the social structure of a collectivity or a group. Social structure is a term that has several different uses in the sociological literature and this is, in part, because of the lack of agreement on how the term social structure should be defined ( Porpora, 1989 ; López and Scott, 2000) . One main dispute pits the dualism of “action” (or agency) vs. “structure” in mainstream sociological work (for a discussion see López and Scott, 2000 ). Consequently, many of the definitions describe behaviors rather than the role of social institutions (e.g., Cortina et al., 2012 ; Tanner and Jackson, 2012 ; Wilson, 2012 ). For example, Tanner and Jackson (2012) define social structure as “the formation of groups via connections among individuals” (p. 260), which focuses on meso-level interactions among individuals. Similarly, Macionis et al. (2008) define social structure as “any relatively stable pattern of social behavior” (p. 13).

The social structure being discussed in the present paper refers to macro-level elements of society such as institutions that facilitate and structure collective interactions, roles or behaviors. Thus, directly inspired from the most prominent definitions of social structure in the literature ( Marx, 1859/1970 ; Giddens, 1979 ; Porpora, 1989 ; López and Scott, 2000 ; Stinchcombe, 2000 ), social structure is defined here as a system of socio-economic stratification, social institutions, organizations, national policies and laws that help structure the norms, roles, behaviors, and values of community members 4 .

In both sociology and psychology, a rupture in the social structure is at the heart of definitions of social change. For example, for Breakwell and Lyons (1996) , changes involve the disintegration of previous national and international order and sets in motion a process of re-definition and re-evaluation of societal norms, belief systems, and power structures. While the communal sense of continuity and permanence is challenged, social change often represents a period of massive transformations in political, social, and economic structures (e.g., Goodwin, 1998 ; Kim and Ng, 2008 ; Chen, 2012 ). This conceptualization is similar to the definition inspired by sociologists and provided by Silbereisen and Tomasik ( 2010 , p. 243) where “social change is understood as a more or less rapid and comprehensive change of societal structures and institutions, including changes to the economic, technological, and cultural frameworks of a society ( Calhoun, 1992 )” or to Kohn's definition of radical social change: “we refer not to the pace of change but to the nature of the change—the transformation of one political and economic system into a quite different system” ( Kohn et al., 1997 , p. 615).

When research focuses on collective action, social structure is placed at the root of their definition. For example, “Breakdown Theories” in sociology argue that social movements result from the disruption or breakdown of previously integrative social structures. This theory regards collective action as a form of social imbalance that results from the improper functioning of social institutions ( Tilly et al., 1975 ). Macionis et al. (2008) also suggest that, “revolutionary social movements attempt to target the whole collectivity by radically changing social institutions” (p. 452). Put differently, for social movements and collective action to occur, social institutions—consequently, the social structure of society—needs to be altered. In other words, social change “is the sudden shifting of power from group to group” ( Schrickel, 1945 , p. 188). To many authors, DSC involves a rupture in the social structure (e.g., Prilleltensky, 1990 ) where people need to “negotiate their way through or around social structures” ( May, 2011 , p. 367).

Rupture in the Normative Structure

The third characteristic of DSC that emerged from the literature is the rupture in the normative structure of society. While reading on the subject, I noticed an important distinction between social structure and normative structure. As mentioned in the previous section, that distinction pointed to a duality that is also observed by theorists in sociology who attempt to define social structure (e.g., Giddens, 1979 ; Mayhew, 1980 ; Porpora, 1989 ; López and Scott, 2000 ). Although both the social and normative structures refer to the functioning of a society, they each point to two different aspects of communities and groups. As discussed earlier, the social structure is associated with macro processes such as social institutions (e.g., Government), whereas the normative structure is related to micro processes as they principally refer to community members' habitual behaviors and norms.

Based on the work of Taylor and de la Sablonnière (2013 , 2014) , the normative structure is defined here as the behaviors of most community members whose aim is achieving collective goals . In other terms, when the normative structure is clear, people know what to do and when to engage in specific behaviors in order to meet the overarching goals of the collectivity. The definition of normative structure also takes its inspiration from an array of different domains in the scientific literature. Mainly, it comes from the definitions of social change that most often involve a change in behaviors and habits that are disrupted with the event of a dramatic and rapid social change. For example, Bishop ( 1998 , p. 406) clearly states that social change in its transformational form refers to “the ability of a group to behave differently, even to creating brand-new elements, within the same social identity.” This definition concurs with definitions of many more authors, such as Delanty's (2012) concept of “normative culture” or May's (2011), where the mundane “ordinary” activities take a central place in social change.

Research and theories on social change have put normative structure as one of its central tenants. For example, Tomasik et al. (2010) , argue that social change involves “changes of the macro-context that disturb habits, interrupt routines, or require novel behaviors relevant for a successful mastery” (p. 247). These authors also assert that when a gradual social change occurs, “old options of thinking and behaving are usually still available whereas abrupt social change is often associated with an immediate blocking of old options” ( Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004 , p. 295). Therefore, in the latter case, it will be necessary to develop new ways of doing things.

Jerneić and Šverko (2001) argue that “major political and socioeconomic changes may strongly influence people's life role priorities, which are otherwise relatively stable behavioral dispositions” (p. 46). In fact, the normative structure of a society is comprised not only of norms and behaviors, but also of roles that people have in their everyday lives. When a DSC occurs, these normative elements of people's lives are all greatly affected to the point where they need to be redefined. Similarly, McDade and Worthman (2004) refer to “socialization ambiguity,” a state present in the context of DSC where “inconsistent messages or conflicting expectations regarding appropriate beliefs and social behavior during the course of socialization may be a substantial source of stress for the developing individual” (p. 52; see also Arnett, 1995 ; Tonkens, 2012 ).

This rupture in the normative structure of society is present not only when radical changes such as natural disasters occur, but also when social change is the result of collective actions within a society. Subašić et al. (2012) acknowledge that “what we do is evidently shaped by social norms, by institutional possibilities, and institutional constraints. But equally, we can act—act together that is—to alter norms, institutions, and even whole social systems” (p. 66). Therefore, when members of a society come together and engage in collective actions, an important aspect of society they aim to change deals with the norms and normative structure.

The importance of the normative component involved in DSC is in accordance with the Normative Theory of Social Change, developed by Taylor et al. ( Taylor and de la Sablonnière, 2013 , 2014 ; see also de la Sablonnière et al., 2009b ). According to their theory, any group—whether it be at the collective, community or country level—functions along the basic 80-20 principle in times of stability. According to this principle, most of the citizens in a functioning society (i.e., 80% of them) will exhibit normative behaviors that agree with the normative structure of the society in order to accomplish collective goals such as achieving a healthy society, and by extension, personal goals such as maintaining a healthy lifestyle. It is the 80% that provide social support, when necessary, to the 20% of citizens who do not function successfully in the society. In theory, as long as there is a decent majority of people who conform to the normative structure, a society should function relatively smoothly. Unfortunately, this is not always the case. Sometimes, when a society is confronted with DSC, its normative structure is ruptured which may lead to societal dysfunction or important disruptions in the “usual” behavior of group members. In such a situation, the amount of group members exhibiting behaviors that are in agreement with the collective goals of the group will be lower than usual. Therefore, it is possible that instead of having 80% of group members acting according to the normative rules of the society, only 30 or 40 % of individuals will follow these rules. In this case, it becomes very difficult for people to restore the functional equilibrium of the normative structure as only a few group members are in a position to provide the necessary social support for the entire society to function properly ( Taylor and de la Sablonnière, 2014 ). What is suggested here is consistent with the work of Albert and Sabini (1974) . These authors refer to the importance of a supportive environment, or social support, which has a sufficient presence in “slow change,” but not when the context is one of rapid change.

Threat to Cultural Identity

The fourth characteristic of social change is threats to the cultural identity of a group. This characteristic is a difficult one to label since different authors use different terms to describe a threat to cultural identity (i.e., lack of clarity, identity conflict, identity crisis, lowered identification, identity confusion). As opposed to terms such as identity conflict, identity crisis, lack of identity clarity and identity change, “threat to cultural identity” was chosen for its capacity to suggest a potential modification in identity. To be considered DSC, the cultural identity in its current form must somehow be jeopardized, challenged, or lowered. Values and beliefs are, per se , questioned and the individual may sense a general lack of clarity and feel threatened to the core of his group identity, value system, or beliefs.

Many scientists have defined and researched collective and/or cultural identity. Recently, Ashmore et al. (2004) have defined collective identity as “first and foremost a statement about categorical membership. A collective identity is one that is shared with a group of others who have (or are believed to have) some characteristic(s) in common” (p. 81). This definition is similar to the one from Taylor (1997) , in which cultural identity is referred to as the beliefs about shared rules and behaviors ( Taylor, 1997 , 2002 ; Usborne and de la Sablonnière, 2014 ).

When a social change occurs, it threatens the cultural identity of all community members. In the present paper, inspired from previous work on cultural identity, I define threat to cultural identity as a serious threat to identification and to the clarity of the shared beliefs, values, attitudes, and behavioral scripts associated with one's group . Throughout the literature I reviewed, cultural identity threat was manifested according to three main themes. The first theme that stood out is that threats to identity are associated with a loss of identity or an identity change (e.g., subtractive identification pattern; de la Sablonnière et al., 2016 ). Some authors directly mention the threat to cultural identity within the context of major social change (e.g., Vaughan, 1986 ; Smelser and Swedberg, 1994 ; Sztompka, 2000 ; Wyn and White, 2000 ; Van Binh, 2002 ; Terry and Jimmieson, 2003 ). For example, in his paper on how cultures change as a function of mass immigration Moghaddam (2012) argues that globalization results in sudden contact among different groups of people from different countries. This form of sudden contact has often resulted in the extinction of many cultures and languages such as Indigenous peoples around the world. Therefore, globalization makes people feel that their collective identity is threatened. Specifically, they experience a loss in many components of their cultural identity including their values and their language (see also Van Binh, 2002 ). The process described by Moghaddam is similar to the one proposed by Lapuz (1976) who argues that when social change occurs rapidly, people's beliefs and values are threatened since the old guidelines are no longer available. One consequence of this threat is that people become confused as values and beliefs contribute to the emotional security and psychological survival of individuals ( Lapuz, 1976 ; Varnum, 2008 ). This is in agreement with Albert's (1977) proposition: “Rapid change constitutes a major threat to self-identity” (p. 499). Similarly, in their book entitled Changing European Identities , Breakwell and Lyons (1996) discuss the mechanisms associated with change in identities in the context of the development of the European Union and refer to a loss of national identity. This change in cultural identity is similar to what Wall and Louchakova (2002) describe as a “shift in the cultural collective consciousness” (p.253). This consists of a change in the American self and the emergence of new selves, more independent and alive in the context of change (see also Neves and Caetano, 2009 ; May, 2011 ).

The second theme is associated with the lack of identity clarity in the event of DSC. This lack of clarity is due to uncertainties or inconsistencies in the definition of one's identity. A clear cultural identity is defined as “the extent to which beliefs about one's group are clearly and confidently defined” ( Usborne and Taylor, 2010 , p. 883; see also Taylor, 2002 ). It has been theorized and demonstrated that an unclear cultural identity can result in lower self-esteem ( Usborne and Taylor, 2010 ). Thus, if the entire collective is experiencing an unclear cultural identity, it may affect people's ability to function effectively in their society. Similarly, Macionis et al. (2008) refer to inconsistencies in the context of socialization in times of important change. People try to seek out new roles, try new “selves” ( Macionis et al., 2008 , p.461). They need to adapt to the inconsistent model their societies are projecting, which leads to “socialization ambiguity” ( McDade and Worthman, 2004 , p. 49). Because social change brings uncertainty in society, it can affect many aspects of individuals' lives such as family relations ( Noak et al., 2001 ), and aspects associated with the self such as “emotions, values, perceptions, identity” ( Wall and Louchakova, 2002 , p. 266).

Finally, as a third theme, authors refer to conflicting identities within the context of dramatic contextual change. For example, Becker conducted a study to find out how rapid social change, such as introducing television in a community that had never owned televisions before, would impact body images of girls and women in that community ( Becker, 2004 ). She found that television caused confusion and conflicts about ideal body images, and consequently “reshap[ed] [their] personal and cultural identities” ( Becker, 2004 , p. 551). In some cases, it even led to eating disorders ( Becker, 2004 ), which has a direct link with the way people evaluate and perceive themselves. In other words, this DSC altered their identity. In fact, severe contextual changes can challenge the meaning of identity and threaten its existence ( Ethier and Deaux, 1994 ; Macek et al., 2013 ). Similarly, Hoffman and Medlock-Klyukovski (2004) argue that contemporary organizations are “typically marked by conflicting interests and contradictory demands on individuals” (p. 389). This is similar to Chen (2012) who refers to the need for a transformation and the need to create new cultural norms and values when confronted to the context of social change ( Chen, 2012 ).

The Typology of Social Change

In order to properly conceptualize DSC and other social contexts associated with the state of a collectivity, I suggest a typology of social change comprised of four different social contexts: “stability,” “inertia,” “incremental social change,” and “DSC” (see Table 1 for definitions). These social contexts are consistent with the theoretical stance of a large number of sociologists (e.g., Durkheim, 1893/1967 , 1897/1967 ; Watzlawick et al., 1974 ; Rocher, 1992 ; Fukuyama, 1999 ; Rogers, 2003 ; May, 2011 ; Nolan and Lenski, 2011 ), psychologists (e.g., Katz, 1974 ; Moghaddam, 2002 ; Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004 ; Goodwin, 2006 ; de la Sablonnière et al., 2009a ) and scientists in the field of organizational behavior (e.g., Golembiewski et al., 1976 ; Tushman and Romanelli, 1985 ; Armenakis et al., 1986 ; Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ; Thompson and Hunt, 1996 ).

As many different concepts surround each of the four social contexts, it was necessary to choose a meaningful label for each. For “stability” and “inertia,” the choice was relatively easy because these two labels are commonly used and applied consistently. The term “status quo” was also considered rather than “stability” (e.g., Prilleltensky, 1990 ; Diekman and Goodfriend, 2007 ; Mucchi-Faina et al., 2010 ). However, because there could also be “status quo” in the context of inertia (e.g., Subašić et al., 2008 ), the term “stability” was preferred.

When it came to “incremental” and “dramatic” social change, the decision was more arduous as authors from different research fields use different labels. For example, instead of referring to “DSC,” Golembiewski et al. (1976) refers to “gamma changes”; Nadler and Tushman (1995) , to “discontinuous change.” Others refer to “second-order change” ( Watzlawick et al., 1974 ; Bartunek and Moch, 1987 ; Bate, 1994 ; Newman, 2000 ), to “abrupt” (e.g., Back, 1971 ; Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004 ) or even to “rapid” change (e.g., Becker, 2004 ; McDade and Worthman, 2004 ). The term “dramatic” social change was chosen for its ability to clearly and distinctively define the situation confronting ordinary people. In a similar fashion, the term “incremental” social change was preferred over the labels: “first-order change,” “beta change,” and “continuous change.”

When there is stability , the actual state of a society is maintained and the majority of group members are actively attempting to attain society's goals. As Weinstein (2010) describes it, it is a state in which “the established order appears to be operating effectively, and disturbing influences from within or from other societies are insignificant” (p. 9; see also Bess (2015) where no change is equated with stability). Indeed, none of the four characteristic of social change are present. For example, the social and normative structures fluctuate little, and changes do not affect what is defined as normal behavior in a community ( Harmon et al., 2015 ). Indeed, personal change, such as bereavement or divorce, still occurs for some members of society. However, in the event of a personal change, the social or normative structures are not disrupted, mainly because the collective social support system remains functional and people can rely on that support in case they experience changes in their individual lives. This is also consistent with the findings of Albert and Sabini (1974) who argue that changes occurring in a supportive environment or in a peripheral element of society are perceived as less disruptive than those occurring in a non-supportive environment because the strain upon society is attenuated.

Consistent with previous research, stability can be defined as a situation where an event, regardless of its pace, does not affect the equilibrium of a society's social and normative structures nor the cultural identity of group members. The event, may, however, impact an isolated number of individuals . An example that might clarify this definition of stability is the event of an election. Although many people can get excited and seem to be affected by this event, an election does not necessarily bring about a rupture in a society, even if it involves a change of political party. The core elements of society remain stable and citizens resume their activities without feeling their lives have been overly disrupted by the election and its outcome. If, for instance, supporters of the defeated party feel sad and hopeless about the defeat, plenty of other citizens will be available to help them cope since most of them will not be affected by the change of government. However, in a different context, the event of an election may trigger DSC; for example, when it leads to a social revolution.

In contrast with stability, a context where there is inertia involves a situation that does affect a large number of people, if not most of the people composing a society. Inertia is defined as a situation where an event, regardless of its pace, does not either reinstate the equilibrium of a society's social and normative structures or clarify the cultural identity of group members .

In times of inertia, if a “positive” event occurs, there is no sustainability to maintain its positive impact. Here, the example of Belarus is used, a country where the population has been in a state of inertia since the fall of the Soviet Union. Lukashenko has been the president of the country since 1994. Under his autocratic rule, Belarus is known as the last dictatorship in Europe. Many Belarusians are longing for a more democratic and open society, yet the country remains in inertia. Buchanan et al. (2005) describe a situation of inertia as an “absence of appropriate activity, a lack of capability, a failure to pay attention to signals, and thus as an impediment rather than a desired condition” (p. 190). Inertia is seen as an undesirable situation where constructive change is not possible because the organization (or the group) does not have the capacity (e.g., lack of resources or will) to carry out the needed change. These authors also argue that when a change is implemented, its sustainability requires managers and staff (or community members) to share the same objectives. Uncertainty about the future must be minimal.

Accordingly, one can assume that the criteria underpinning sustainability in the event of a change are already absent in a society that has stagnated due to inertia. Therefore, inertia in a society such as Belarus constitutes a context where the population is uncertain about the future and does not share the same long-term goals as its government. There is a desire for positive social change, but the actual structure of the society makes it difficult for any change to be implemented and be sustained. Indeed, for a positive change to be maintained, it must have the support of individuals in power since they have the appropriate resources to address society's problems. Unsurprisingly, sustainability of such a change is threatened by an autocratic style of governing ( Buchanan et al., 2005 ).

In sum, inertia differs from stability. In the case of inertia, most members of society desire a change from the actual state of their group, but are unable to properly sustain change due to a lack of collective social support and an unclear cultural identity. In contrast, in the case of “stability,” the society functions in an efficient manner when meeting the collective goals.

Incremental Social Change

Incremental social change is defined as a situation where a slow event leads to a gradual but profound societal transformation and slowly changes the social and/or the normative structure or changes/threatens the cultural identity of group members . The slow pace is necessary for incremental social change to occur. Moreover, at least one of the other three characteristics needs to occur. In their recent paper, Abrams and Vasiljevic (2014) speak of “growth,” which could represent one form of incremental social change that involves “wider acceptance of shared values and tolerance of different values” and of “recession” where “disidentification” with current groups can occur (p. 328).

One of the most cited examples of incremental social change is technological innovation (e.g., Rieger, 2003 ; Weinstein, 2010 ; May, 2011 ; Hansen et al., 2012 ). Often, there is no social structural rupture associated with the wide use of technology and normative structure as well as social support remain intact. Given its incremental nature, this type of social change does not instantly produce conflict between old and new behaviors. For instance, when television was introduced, people bought it without knowing the consequences of the implementation of this new technology in their life ( Becker, 2004 ; Macionis et al., 2008 ; Weinstein, 2010 ). Today, in retrospect, we know that buying a television set entailed a plethora of new behaviors that altered our society and our way of living. Indeed, some changes in society seem to be a “by-product of our pursuit of other goals and interests” ( Subašić et al., 2012 , p. 62). The long time span that is typical for incremental social change makes its outcomes unpredictable and unintentional. For instance, as Weinstein states ( Weinstein, 2010 ), “It would be impossible to assess exactly what role electronic telecommunication has played in our global revolution, in part because its effects continue to reverberate and magnify as you read this” (p. 4).

The cell phone is a particularly good example of incremental social change. When it came onto the buyer's market, only a few exclusive people possessed one. However, over the years, it became increasingly normative to have a cell phone and, today, it is almost inconceivable not to have one. Furthermore, when cell phones were first marketed, they were used mainly for business rather than for social purposes, which is the current primary use ( Aoki and Downes, 2003 ). In the same vein, other technological changes, such as the emergence of personal computers ( Kiesler et al., 1984 ; Robinson et al., 1997 ), Internet ( DiMaggio et al., 2001 ; Brignall III and Van Valey, 2005 ), and social media ( Robinson et al., 1997 ; O'Keeffe and Clarke-Pearson, 2011 ; Oh et al., 2015 ) will, in the future, be recognized as key events in the historical transformation of social structures and social norms. Such technology does not represent a DSC, but a social change nonetheless as it has modified the way people interact with one another in an incremental manner. As the change occurs for a relatively long period of time, there is consistency in the pattern of change, which allows social structures to adapt and, thus, to remain intact ( Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ). Individuals experiencing incremental social change are therefore able to adapt, given that the collective social support is not altered. For example, there is support for people that have yet to possess a cell phone; if they want to buy one, but do not understand how it functions, there are plenty of people that can help them adapt to this new technology. Even if technological change is conceptualized here as an incremental change, it is possible that technology is used to provoke a DSC, for example by instigating an important social revolution ( Rodriguez, 2013 ).

Despite technology being the most adequate example, other incremental changes can be observed in other aspects of society such as in medicine. Indeed, advancement in medicine such as effective birth control ( Goldin and Katz, 2002 ) was also the cause of a profound incremental social change. The example of contraception is crucial as the pill deeply affected gender roles in society by empowering women by giving them the capacity to control their sexuality. The pill had not only direct positive effects on women's career investments, but also on the opportunity of attending school longer. The pill forever changed women's involvement in our societies and the repercussions of this incremental social change still echo to us through struggles for gender equality, but also in the form of women actively involved in every level of the modern workplace, including higher managements and governmental position. In other words, the gradual nature of incremental social change makes it a profound change in society that neither disturbs the social structure nor the collective social support system.

Dramatic Social Change

DSC has been defined as “profound societal transformations that produce a complete rupture in the equilibrium of social structures because their adaptive capacities are surpassed” ( de la Sablonnière et al., 2009a , p. 325). Although this definition is based on previous sociological work ( Parsons, 1964 ; Rocher, 1992 ), it is adapted here according to the four characteristic of DSC. Specifically, I suggest that DSC be defined as a situation where a rapid event leads to a profound societal transformation and produces a rupture in the equilibrium of the social and normative structures and changes/threatens the cultural identity of group members .

As with incremental change, DSC induces fundamental transformations in society. However, the shift occurs at a much more rapid pace, provoking a break with the past. Some authors have highlighted this sense of discontinuity by referring to DSC as the disintegration of a previous social order or as the break in a frame of reference ( Golembiewski et al., 1976 ; Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ; Breakwell and Lyons, 1996 ). They also use terms such as the “construction of something new,” a “reconceptualization,” or a “re-definition.” Indeed, the breakdown of a social structure conveys the need for the reconstruction of core elements in a society. Accordingly, DSC can be conceptualized as a complete rupture in the social structure that marks the end of one period and the beginning of another one, or where a type of society is transformed into another ( Tushman and Romanelli, 1985 ; Kohn et al., 2000 ; Weinstein, 2010 ). Other researchers, such as Rogers (2003) , also see rapid social change as intertwined with the social structure. More specifically, Rogers (2003) states that rapid social change can threaten social structure by surpassing the adaptive capacities of individuals. Unsurprisingly, DSC is the most disruptive type of change not only for the social structure but also for the majority of society members experiencing it, i.e., the normative structure as well as cultural identities are challenged. As DSC entails a re-definition of values, norms and relations, individuals can no longer rely on their habits and routine; they need to learn new skills and new definitions and more challengingly, unlearn the old ways of doing things ( Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ; Tomasik et al., 2010 ). Consequently, DSC is described as a painful and confusing experience for individuals ( Hinkle, 1952 ; Lapuz, 1976 ; Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ; Kohn et al., 2000 ; Wall and Louchakova, 2002 ; Rioufol, 2004 ; Hegmon et al., 2008 ).

A good example of DSC is the breakdown of the Soviet Union. If I return to Zoia's example, it is clear that all the people in Kyrgyzstan and in the Former Soviet Union were affected by the breakdown of the Soviet Union. Zoia is not the only one who lost all her savings: the vast majority of people lost their savings within a matter of days. In terms of social support, whom could she have relied on if all of her friends were also in the same situation? Regarding to the fall of the former Soviet Union, Goodwin (2006) argues that older people were inclined to receive less social support in part because the majority of the population, including family members, were struggling with several jobs just to provide themselves with basic needs. Furthermore, elderly citizens could not even rely on formal social services because the collapse of the former Soviet Union caused a decline in formal state support, which left them no time to rebuild their retirement income. This illustrates the rupture in the structure of society that can be found when a DSC occurs as well as the effect on the majority of ordinary group members who cannot rely on collective social support.

Coming Full Circle: Theoretical Implications

Heraclitus, an ancient Greek philosopher, is credited for saying that “the only thing constant is change.” Gradually or within an instant, civilizations, societies, communities or organizations that often seem immutable face multiple DSCs. Social scientists agree that social changes are not only intensifying but also defining today's world. In fact, Weinstein (2010) has underscored that “rapid change, both peaceful and violent, is a fact of life that virtually everyone on Earth today has come to expect, if not unconditionally accept” (p. 3).

For the present paper, my aim was to initiate a conversation about the psychology of social change. Thus, I briefly reviewed the major perspectives of social change in both sociology and psychology. Research conducted in both fields and their subfields have remained in distinct silos with no effort made toward aggregating their findings. This has unfortunately resulted in the absence of an encompassing approach in the current literature of social change: social change has never been integrated into a single perspective that would define or contextualize DSC within the spectrum of different social contexts. More importantly, social change has not been conceptualized so that micro processes, macro processes, and the important relations between them are addressed. As a result, the typology of social change introduces different social contexts (e.g., stability) that can serve as a basis of comparison for DSC. Based on my review of the literature, I suggest four necessary characteristics of DSC (Table 6 ).

The present paper then offers a first step toward unifying the variety of theories of social change which are currently isolated from each other. Indeed, our approach aims at addressing the challenge raised by Sun and Ryder (2016) concerning our need for “a more nuanced understanding of rapid sociocultural change combined with sophisticated research methods designed to address change in a multilevel way” (p. 9). The typology of social change I am suggesting is an emerging concept; thus, I invite debate with the hope that the views presented here will stimulate others to contribute to a needed understanding of DSC within an individual perspective. More importantly, based on such a typology of social change, theoretical models could be suggested as they might offer a guide to understanding the consequences of social change. For instance, such theoretical models could answer these three questions: Are the different social contexts associated with one-another? What makes a society move from one social context to another (e.g., from stability to DSC)? What is the role of the different characteristics of DSC? So far, answers to the three questions raised above were left lingering and the different characteristics of DSC were not arranged in a sequential way nor were they identified as key movers of one state of society to another. In Figure 1 , I offer a theoretical model that integrates the social contexts and the characteristics of DSC as a first step toward a psychology of social change.

As seen in Figure 1 , neither a slow nor a fast pace event will influence the status quo in both stability and inertia. There will therefore be no break with the past and so no rupture in the social and normative structures. Thus, in these two social contexts, if an event were to occur rapidly, the current situation of a group or society would remain unaffected by it; that is why pace is not the only characteristic important to define DSC. For example, if a plane crashes, which is a rapidly occurring dramatic event, it does not necessarily affect an entire community. Also, in a state of stability, when a fast—or slow—event takes place, because the normative and the social structures are unaffected, there is no direct threat to the group's cultural identity. Similarly, when an event occurs in a state of inertia, there is no additional threat to the society's cultural identity, because the normative and social structure are unaffected.

In contrast, in a state of incremental social change, slow-occurring events, if profound enough, will gradually change the social and normative structures, as well as threaten or change cultural identity. For a DSC to occur, a fast event needs to take place. If that event has enough impact—therefore not in a state of stability or inertia—, it will rupture the social structure and the normative structures. As shown by many different DSC contexts, there are three possible scenarios when it comes to the rupture of these two structures: (1) the social structure ruptures first, which later leads to the rupture of the normative structure (e.g., Zhang and Hwang, 2007 ), (2) the normative structure ruptures first, which later leads to the rupture of the social structure (e.g., Centola and Baronchelli, 2015 ), or (3) both the social and normative structures rupture simultaneously and influence each other.

An example of the first scenario would be the latest presidential elections in the United States. The recent proclamation of Donald Trump as president carries the potential for political transformations as well as changes in the United States' economic structure (rupture to social structure). The leadership of Trump's administration can carry major structural change that would then lead to a rupture of the normative structure. At this point, there are indications that this new governance (social structure) may very well affect the normative structure. Some members of the population have become more “open” to expressing their reluctance to have more immigrants come to the USA, which could eventually lead to a rupture in normative structure where different ethnic groups overtly fight each other within America. A second example was the loss of the French Canadians to the English Canadians at the Battle of the Plains of Abraham in 1759. This battle was a pivotal moment in the 7 Years' War and gave power to the British troops ( Veyssière, 2013 ). The result of the battle culminated in the French losing most of their economical structural powers to the English and the start of a decline of education. Consequently, the French mentality and behaviors were modified. The norms had to be adapted to new rules and to the loss of economic power ( Veyssière, 2013 ).

The normative structure can rupture before the social structure in situations such as the African-American Civil Rights Movement in the United-States, the Fall of Apartheid in South Africa, or the Quiet Revolution in Québec. If in the past African-Americans were afflicted by a sense of resignation, leaders such as Martin Luther King Jr. and Rosa Parks gave them the will they needed to fight for a better future for themselves. This rupture in the normative structure led to the African-American Civil Rights Movement which, in turn, brought about changes to the social structure (e.g., School desegregation). This movement against racial inequality, segregation and discrimination instigated the Civil Rights Act of 1964, which banned any type of segregation based on race, color, religion or sex, as well as other changes in federal legislation.

The breakdown of the Soviet Union is an example that can be used to illustrate a simultaneous rupture of the social and normative structures. This event caused major transformations in the economic, political, and social structures (rupture to social structure). Simultaneously, a large proportion of the population found themselves in a great economic crisis, which led to disruptions in their usual behaviors and habits, such as working multiple jobs instead of just one (rupture of normative structures).

When the normative and the social structures are ruptured (regardless of the order in which this occurs), cultural identity will be threatened. There will be a global sense of confusion, ambiguity, and lack of clarity that might motivate individual group members to change their identification with their group.

Depending on society's and the individual's abilities to cope, there are two possible outcomes: stability or inertia. If the society in which DSC has taken place is able to develop coping and adaptation mechanisms—both at the individual and societal levels—stability might be restored. Stability would then be achieved when the social and normative structures however different are brought back to functionality and when cultural identity is clear and no longer under threat. In contrast, if the society and individuals are not able to develop coping mechanisms, society might enter a state of inertia. In inertia, even though a society in a state of inertia is no longer going through major social changes, the need or desire for change still lingers ( Sloutsky and Searle-White, 1993 ). This can be due to a DSC that did not, in the end, really change the way a collectivity is ruled or how its citizens are treated ( Moghaddam and Crystal, 1997 ; Moghaddam and Lvina, 2002 ).

Consequences of DSC

Knowing about the range of different social contexts such as stability, inertia, incremental change, and DSC as well as the specific characteristics of DSC, has the potential to guide researchers in terms of assessing DSC and its impact on the psychological well-being of ordinary group members. Specifically, after establishing a clear typology of social change, including potential theoretical models, it is now possible to move on to the second step of the psychology of social change. In this second step, we need to address whether and how different coping mechanisms determine (mediate, moderate) the influence of DSC on psychological well-being. This question goes hand in hand with the work of Norris et al. (2002) who reviewed 160 studies involving natural disasters, mass violence, and technological disasters. They concluded from more than 60,000 participants that such events have negative repercussions on participants' lives. In most of the research they report, social support, economic status, and age were the identified factors that may be associated with a better adaptation to social change. Although diverse factors were suggested, the research they reported was “atheoretical and little of it is programmatic” ( Norris et al., 2002 , p. 249). In accordance with Norris et al. (2002) , I argue that the mediators or moderators involved in adaptation mechanisms should become the focus of future studies. The four characteristics I have identified have the potential to become pivotal in meeting this objective. In sum, the link between social change and well-being is still unclear (e.g., Liu et al., 2014 ; Sun and Ryder, 2016 ). Such an investigation could eventually guide us in designing concrete interventions to help people adapt to the challenges of DSC ( Rogers, 2003 ; Vago, 2004 ).

The concept of resilience emerges from the literature as potentially useful for understanding people's coping mechanisms. Resilience is defined as the act of bouncing back in the face of adversity ( Bonanno, 2004 ). For the specific example of DSC, resilient individuals would be those who have been able to maintain their normal functioning and adapt themselves to adverse situations ( Masten, 2001 ; Curtis and Cicchetti, 2003 ; Luthar, 2003 ; Masten and Powell, 2003 ). Research has shown that a significant number of people are able to adapt to challenging personal situations (e.g., Bonanno, 2004 ). However, resilience has mostly been studied within the context of personal changes such as the death of a loved one or a personal trauma ( Bonanno, 2004 ). Similar to a personal change, this variation in reactions may be due to individual differences in resilience. This highlights the need to consider this variable within the psychology of social change. More concretely, the literature on resilience may prove to be important when linking people's perceptions of the characteristic of DSC to the various paths of recovery (e.g., resilience, recovery, chronic distress, and delayed reactions; Bonanno, 2004 ).

While most research on resilience focuses on “personal events,” there is, however, another type of resilience known as “collective resilience” or “community resilience” (e.g., Landau and Saul, 2004 ; Kirmayer et al., 2011 ) which may be more relevant in the context of DSC as the concept hints that the majority of society is affected by the change. To illustrate collective resilience, let us consider the case where the normative structure of a society is dissolved and its cultural identity is threatened. Individuals in this situation would no longer have guidelines and values to individually cope with DSC. Moreover, every individual affected by the change would be in the same negative situation. Consequently, individuals might need to find ways to collectively adapt to the transformations. The processes associated with resilience may thus differ in situations of personal vs. social change. I therefore believe it is important to explore whether the adaptation mechanisms are the same in a context of DSC where social support is not readily available.

Conducting Research on Social Change

In order to speak of a real psychology of social change, we must be able to actually study social change and its consequences. The use of a mix of methodologies that would include large correlational or longitudinal surveys conducted in the field as well as laboratory experiments ( de la Sablonnière et al., 2013 ; see also Liu and Bernardo, 2014 ; Sun and Ryder, 2016 ) might prove to be the only way to truly study social change and its consequences. On the one hand, correlational designs conducted in the field are necessary to capture people's firsthand experience with DSC. They are however limited by their design that prevents claims of causality. They are also known to be demanding in terms of both human and financial resources, and may well be dangerous at times for researchers. Moreover, they require an intimate knowledge of the culture such as the language as well as contacts within the community to facilitate the research and collaboration process.

On the other hand, laboratory experiments are necessary to establish the controlled conditions needed to understand associations between the characteristics of social change and the consequences. Laboratory experiments, however, are difficult to design, because it is a challenge to reproduce the actual characteristics of social change in the laboratory which limits their ecological validity ( de la Sablonnière et al., 2013 ). Indeed, social change typically entails various elements such as historical processes, a collective perspective, and associated cultural elements ( Moghaddam and Crystal, 1997 ) which must be taken into consideration in order to replicate their impact in an artificial setting. For example, the impact of the Tohoku tsunami in Japan or the Syrian conflict cannot be recreated in their entirety in a laboratory; nor can all the characteristic of social change be taken into consideration in a laboratory study designed to assess the impact(s) of social change. However, if an array of studies using different characteristic of DSC were to be conducted (or a combination of multiple characteristic), the convergence of the results would make us able to better understand and thereby predict the impact of DSC on individuals and communities. At the very least in a laboratory, researchers can expose participants to imagined changes through a scenario or a video that would include, in the experimental condition, one or more of the four characteristics of DSC (Pelletier-Dumas et al., submitted). If the scientific community accepts that experimental studies will not exactly mirror DSC, but instead test some of the characteristics in a large number of experiments, there is potential for laboratory experiments to bring an important contribution that would eventually allow a generalization to the real world (for examples see Betsch et al., 2015 ; Caldwell et al., 2016 ; Pelletier-Dumas et al., submitted).

The difficulties of conducting research on social change are, however, amplified by the challenge of obtaining ethical consent in a manner that allows for timely research. In terms of experimental manipulations of DSC, obtaining the ethical board's consent can be tedious. Indeed, according to some authors ( Kelman, 1967 ; Bok, 1999 ; Clarke, 1999 ; Herrera, 1999 ; Pittenger, 2002 ) deceiving participants is difficult to justify ethically. This objection on the use of deception can undermine any attempt to seriously study DSC, as deception can be a valuable methodological asset ( Bortolotti and Mameli, 2006 ), especially with such an elusive subject. Furthermore, research on new grounds require new techniques and methods on which ethicists can put limits, to ensure that they do not cause harm to participants ( Root Wolpe, 2006 ). As with any new technology, methods focused on inducing dramatic-like changes can be perceived as having unsuspected risks.

In order to truly understand the interplay between individuals and their context, social psychological theories must take into account that we live in a constantly changing world. Unfortunately, although social psychology was rooted in understanding social change, most modern psychological theories refrain from addressing a “true” psychology of social change and prefer relegating social change to the field of sociology.

Through increasing the focus on social change, we could combine, on the one hand, sociology's emphasis on the importance of social change with, on the other hand, psychology's emphasis on the importance of complex individual processes. As a result, my theoretical proposal aims at bringing together sociology, where social change is central, and psychology, where rigorous scientific methods allow us to study the psychological processes of individuals living in changing social contexts.

In general, more research on the concept of social change is needed so that we can help predict, prevent, and minimize the negative impact of social change. If psychologists and sociologists work together to move toward developing a psychology of social change, perhaps we could come to better understand and help people, like Zoia, who lost almost everything they had, consequently improving the quality of millions of lives experiencing DSC.

Author Contributions

RdlS thought and developed the ideas, as well as wrote the article as sole author. Research assistants were paid to find and read the abstracts of all articles reviewed in this manuscript.

This research was founded by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities research Council of Canada (SSHRC) and by a grant from the Fonds de recherche du Québec – Société et culture (FRQSC).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

RdlS Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal. I wish to thank all my colleagues and the members of the Social Change and Identity Lab for their comments and help. They have heard me talk about social change for the last 10 years and have never stopped encouraging me to pursue these ideas. I am also grateful to all the “Baboushkas” and the people I have met in contexts of DSC. These people continue to inspire me every day. I am grateful to the editor and the three evaluators for their insightful comments. I would also like to thank Matthew Davidson, Saltanat Sadykova, Lily Trudeau-Guévin, Alexie Gendron, Jérémie Dupuis, Raphaël Froment, and Donald M. Taylor for their help during different steps of the preparation of this manuscript. Finally, I want to thank Nada Kadhim who was patient enough to coordinate the material and the team—including me—at all stages.

1. ^ Key sociology readings, such as Human Societies: An Introduction to Macrosociology ( Nolan and Lenski, 2011 ), The Sociology of Social Change ( Sztompka, 1993 ), and Social Change ( Weinstein, 2010 ), offer an in-depth description of these theories that were beyond the scope of the present review.

2. ^ For space limitation, all methodological details and steps I encountered are not discussed here, but are available upon request to the author.

3. ^ The term “social change” must be distinguished from the term “event.” An event may or may not be considered as social change. An event has the potential to bring social change ( Sewell, 1996 ), be it incremental or dramatic. However, an event is not always tied to social change as it may represent a form of “happening” that does not influence the course of history ( Nisbet, 1972 ). In sum, an event is an intrusion or “disturbances, however mild, of the normal” (p. 26). In contrast to social change, with the event, the disruption of the normal might only be temporary and not significant in time.

4. ^ Defining social structure represents a challenge that goes beyond the scope of the present paper. From my understanding of the literature, there are as many conceptions of social structure as there are scientists working on that concept. The most important issue that demonstrates how hard it is to define social structure is the fact that one of the most prominent sociologists, Giddens (1979) , refers to a “duality of structure” when defining social structure (structure vs. agency). On the one hand, social structure represents institutions or more specifically “collective rules and resources that structure behavior” ( Porpora, 1989 , p. 195). Here, scientists refer to “groups, institutions, laws, population characteristics, and set of social relations that form the environment of the organization” ( Stinchcombe, 2000 , p. 142), or to “Lawlike regularities that govern the behavior of social facts” ( Porpora, 1989 , p. 195). On the other hand, social structure represents “the underlying regularities or patterns in how people behave and in their relationships with one another” (i.e., agency; Giddens et al., 2011 , p. 3). Here, the definitions often described normative behaviors or the roles of individuals rather than the role played by social institutions (e.g., Cortina et al., 2012 ; Homans, 1951 ; Mayhew, 1980 ; Tanner and Jackson, 2012 ; Wilson, 2012 ).

This duality lunched a debate in sociology that was reflected not only in Gidden's work but also in others sociologists that have devoted their writings to defining social structure (e.g., Parsons, 1964 ; Mayhew, 1980 ). For example, Porpora (1989) reports four principal ways of conceptualizing social structure that reflect either of these conceptions. More recently, expending on the work of Bourdieu (1975) and of Goffman (1983) , López and Scott (2000) proposed that there is another aspect of social structure that must also be considered in addition to the institutional and relational structures: the embodied structure described as the “habits and skills that are inscribed in human bodies and minds” (p. 4).

To add to that complexity, some researchers (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, 1979 , 1994 ; for other “system views” see for example Marx, 1859/1970 ; Habermas, 1987 ) describe the possible “systems” that are, like Russian dolls, embedded in each other. These systems include the ecological environments “conceived as a set of nested structures” ( Bronfenbrenner, 1994 , p. 39): the microsystems, the mesosystems, the exosystems, the macrosystems, and the chronosystems. This “ecological model” illustrates the complexity of social structure as a sociological term.

Because of the lack of clarity, or maybe because the definition of social structure points to different aspects of the social structure, scientists often avoid defining social structure in their papers, and thereby contribute to the general confusion. Not that the other aspects or levels of social structure are not important (e.g., meso, micro), but the social structure being discussed in the present paper refers exclusively to macro-level elements of society such as institutions and other environmental factors that help facilitate and structure collective interactions, norms, roles, and behaviors.

Abrams, D., and Vasiljevic, M. (2014). How does macroeconomic change affect social identity (and vice versa?): insights from the European context. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 14, 311–338. doi: 10.1111/asap.12052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albert, S. (1977). Temporal comparison theory. Psychol. Rev. 84, 485–503. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.6.485

Albert, S., and Sabini, J. (1974). Attributions about systems in slow vs. rapid change. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1, 91–93. doi: 10.1177/014616727400100131

Amiot, C., de la Sablonnière, R., Terry, D., and Smith, J. (2007). Integration of social identities in the self: toward a cognitive-developmental model. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 11, 364–388. doi: 10.1177/1088868307304091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, C., Törnberg, A., and Törnberg, P. (2014). An evolutionary developmental approach to cultural evolution. Curr. Anthropol. 55, 154–174. doi: 10.1086/675692

Aoki, K., and Downes, E. J. (2003). An analysis of young people's use of and attitudes toward cell phones. Telematics Inform. 20, 349–364. doi: 10.1016/S0736-5853(03)00018-2

Armenakis, A. A., Buckley, M. R., and Bedeian, A. G. (1986). Survey research measurement issues in evaluating change: a laboratory investigation. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 10, 147–157. doi: 10.1177/014662168601000204

Arnett, J. J. (1995). Adolescents' uses of media for self-socialization. J. Youth Adolesc. 24, 519–533. doi: 10.1007/BF01537054

Ashmore, R. D., Deaux, K., and McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2004). An organizing framework for collective identity: articulation and significance of multidimensionality. Psychol. Bull. 130, 80–114. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.1.80

Back, K. W. (1971). Biological models of social change. Am. Sociol. Rev. 36, 660–667. doi: 10.2307/2093596

Bartunek, J. M., and Moch, M. K. (1987). First-order, second-order, and third-order change and organization development interventions: a cognitive approach. J. Appl. Behav. Sci. 23, 483–500. doi: 10.1177/002188638702300404

Bate, S. P. (1994). Strategies for Cultural Change . Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann.

Google Scholar

Batel, S., and Castro, P. (2015). Collective action and social change: examining the role of representation in the communication between protesters and third-party members. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 25, 249–263. doi: 10.1002/casp.2214

Bauman, Z. (2003). Liquid Love: On the Frailty of Human Bonds . Cambridge: Polity Press.

Becker, A. E. (2004). Television, disordered eating, and young women in Fiji: negotiating body image and identity during rapid social change. Cult. Med. Psychiatry 28, 533–559. doi: 10.1007/s11013-004-1067-5

Benet-Martínez, V., and Haritatos, J. (2005). Bicultural identity integration (BII): components and psychosocial antecedents. J. Pers. 73, 1015–1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00337.x

Bernstrøm, V. H., and Kjekshus, L. E. (2015). Effect of organisational change type and frequency on long-term sickness absence in hospitals. J. Nurs. Manag. 23, 813–822. doi: 10.1111/jonm.12218

Berry, J. W. (2005). Acculturation: living successfully in two cultures. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 29, 697–712. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.07.013

Bess, K. D. (2015). The impact of everyday experiences on planned organizational change: applying schematic change theory to the study of narratives in community-based organizations. J. Community Psychol. 43, 739–759. doi: 10.1002/jcop.21757

Betsch, T., Lindow, S., Engel, C., Ulshöfer, C., and Kleber, J. (2015). Has the world changed? My neighbor might know: effects of social context on routine deviation. J. Behav. Decis. Making 28, 50–66. doi: 10.1002/bdm.1828

Bishop, P. (1998). Social change and future practice. Am. Behav. Sci. 42, 406–412. doi: 10.1177/0002764298042003011

Blackwood, L., Livingstone, A. G., and Leach, C. W. (2013). Regarding societal change. J. Soc. Polit. Psychol. 1, 105–111. doi: 10.5964/jspp.v1i1.282

Bok, S. (1999). Lying: Moral Choice in Public and Private Life . New York, NY: Vintage.

Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? Am. Psychol. 59, 20–28. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.20

Bortolotti, L., and Mameli, M. (2006). Deception in psychology: mora; costs and benefits of unsought self-knowledge. Account. Res. 13, 259–275. doi: 10.1080/08989620600848561

Bourdieu, P. (1975). Structures sociales et structures de perception du monde social [Social structures and structures of perception of the social world]. Actes Rech. Sci. Soc. 1, 18–20. doi: 10.3406/arss.1975.3507

CrossRef Full Text

Breakwell, G. M. (1986). Coping with Threatened Identities . London: Methuen.

Breakwell, G. M., and Lyons, E. (1996). Changing European Identities: Social Psychological Analyses of Social Change . Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann.

Brignall III, T. W., and Van Valey, T. V. (2005). The impact of Internet communications on social interaction. Sociol. Spectr. 25, 335–348. doi: 10.1080/02732170590925882

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1994). “Ecological models of human development,” in International Encyclopedia of Education, 2nd Edn ., Vol. 3, eds T. Husén and T. N. Postlethwaite (Oxford: Elsevier), 1643–1647.

Bruscella, J. (2015). Holiday greeting rituals as expressions of ambivalence and indifference toward social change. West. J. Commun. 79, 116–132. doi: 10.1080/10570314.2014.943427

Buchanan, D., Fitzgerald, L., Ketley, D., Gollop, R., Jones, J. L., Lamont, S. S., et al. (2005). No going back: a review of the literature on sustaining organizational change. Int. J. Manag. Rev. 7, 189–205. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2370.2005.00111.x

Burke, W. W., and Litwin, G. H. (1992). A causal model of organizational performance and change. J. Manag. 18, 523–545. doi: 10.1177/014920639201800306

Caldwell, C. A., Atkinson, M., and Renner, E. (2016). Experimental approaches to studying cumulative cultural evolution. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 25, 191–195. doi: 10.1177/0963721416641049

Calhoun, C. (1992). “Social change,” in Encyclopedia of Sociology , Vol. 4, eds E. F. Borgatta and R. J. V. Montgomery (New York, NY: MacMillan), 1807–1812.

Calhoun, C. (2000). “Social change,” in Encyclopedia of Sociology, 2nd Edn ., Vol. 4, eds E. F. Borgatta and R. J. V. Montgomery (New York, NY: MacMillan), 2641–2649.

Centola, D., and Baronchelli, A. (2015). The spontaneous emergence of conventions: an experimental study of structural evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 1989–1994. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418838112

Chen, X. (2012). Human development in the context of social change: introduction. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 321–325. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00259.x

Chen, X. (2015). Exploring the implications of social change for human development: perspectives, issues and future directions. Int. J. Psychol. 50, 56–59. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12128

Chirot, D., and Merton, R. M. (1986). Social Change in the Modern Era . San Diego, CA: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Clarke, S. (1999). Justifying deception in social science research. J. Appl. Philos. 16, 151–166. doi: 10.1111/1468-5930.00117

Comte, A. I. (1853/1929). Système de Politique Positive [System of Positive Polity], Vol. 3 . Paris: Carilian-Goeury et Dalmont.

Cortina, L. M., Curtin, N., and Stewart, A. J. (2012). Where is social structure in personality research? A feminist analysis of publication trends. Psychol. Women Q. 36, 259–273. doi: 10.1177/0361684312448056

Coughlin, B. J., and Khinduka, S. K. (1976). Social change and social action. J. Sociol. Soc. Welf. 3, 322–331.

Crockett, L. J., and Silbereisen, R. K. (Eds.). (2000). Negotiating Adolescence in Times of Social Change . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Curtis, W. J., and Cicchetti, D. (2003). Moving research on resilience into the 21st century: theoretical and methodological considerations in examining the biological contributors to resilience. Dev. Psychopathol. 15, 773–810. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000373

Dadrian, V. N. (1989). Genocide as a problem of national and international law: the World War I Armenian case and its contemporary legal ramifications. Yale J. Int. Law 12, 223–326.

Dadrian, V. N. (1998). The historical and legal interconnections between the Armenian genocide and the Jewish Holocaust: from impunity to retributive justice. Yale J. Int. Law 23, 503–560.

de la Sablonnière, R., Amiot, C. E., Cárdenas, D., Sadykova, N., Gorborukova, G. L., and Huberdeau, M.-E. (2016). Testing the subtractive pattern of cultural identification. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 46, 441–454. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2178

de la Sablonnière, R., and Tougas, F. (2008). Relative deprivation and social identity in times of dramatic social change: the case of nurses. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 38, 2293–2314. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00392.x

de la Sablonnière, R., and Usborne, E. (2014). “Toward a social psychology of social change: insights from Identity Process Theory,” in Identity Process Theory: Identity, Social Action and Social Change , eds R. Jaspal and G. M. Breakwell (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 203–221.

de la Sablonnière, R., Auger, É., Sadykova, N., and Taylor, D. M. (2010). When the “we” impacts how “I” feel about myself: effect of temporal collective relative deprivation on personal well-being in the context of dramatic social change in Kyrgyzstan. Eur. Psychol. 15, 271–282. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000062

de la Sablonnière, R., French Bourgeois, L., and Najih, M. (2013). Dramatic social change: a social psychological perspective. J. Soc. Polit. Psychol. 1, 253–272. doi: 10.5964/jspp.v1i1.14

de la Sablonnière, R., Taylor, D. M., and Sadykova, N. (2009b). Challenges of applying a student-centered approach to learning in the context of education in Kyrgyzstan. Int. J. Educ. Dev. 29, 628–634. doi: 10.1016/j.ijedudev.2009.01.001

de la Sablonnière, R., Taylor, D. M., Perozzo, C., and Sadykova, N. (2009a). Reconceptualizing relative deprivation in the context of dramatic social change: the challenge confronting the people of Kyrgyzstan. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 39, 325–345. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.519

de la Sablonnière, R., Tougas, F., and Lortie-Lussier, M. (2009c). Dramatic social change in Russia and Mongolia: connecting relative deprivation to social identity. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 40, 327–348. doi: 10.1177/0022022108330986

de Lemus, S., and Stroebe, K. (2015). Achieving social change: a matter of all for one? J. Soc. Issues 71, 441–452. doi: 10.1111/josi.12122

Delanty, G. (2012). A cosmopolitan approach to the explanation of social change: social mechanisms, processes, modernity. Sociol. Rev. 60, 333–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-954X.2012.02076.x

Des Forges, A. (1999). Leave None to Tell the Story: Genocide in Rwanda . New York, NY: Human Rights Watch. Available online at: https://www.hrw.org/reports/pdfs/r/rwanda/rwanda993.pdf

Diamond, J. (2005). Collapse: How Societies Choose to Fail or Succeed . New York, NY: Penguin Books.

Diekman, A. B., and Goodfriend, W. (2007). The good and bad of social change: ambivalence toward activist groups. Soc. Justice Res. 20, 401–417. doi: 10.1007/s11211-007-0050-z

DiMaggio, P., Hargittai, E., Neuman, W. R., and Robinson, J. P. (2001). Social implications of the Internet. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 27, 307–336. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.307

Durkheim, É. (1893/1967). De la Division du Travail Social [The Division of Labor in Society] . Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

Durkheim, É. (1897/1967). Le Suicide: Étude de Sociologie [Suicide: A Study in Sociology]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

Ethier, K. A., and Deaux, K. (1994). Negotiating social identity when contexts change: maintaining identification and responding to threat. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 243–251. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.243

Feldman, M. W., and Laland, K. N. (1996). Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. Trends Ecol. Evol. 11, 453–457. doi: 10.1016/0169-5347(96)10052-5

Fried, M. (1964). Effects of social change on mental health. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 34, 3–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1964.tb02187.x

Fukuyama, F. (1999). The Great Disruption: Human Nature and the Reconstitution of Social Order . New York, NY: Free Press.

Giddens, A. (1979). Central Problems in Social Theory: Action, Structure, and Contradiction in Social Analysis . Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Giddens, A., Duneier, M., Appelbaum, R. P., and Carr, D. (2011). Introduction to Sociology, 8th Edn . New York, NY: Norton Publishing.

Goffman, E. (1983). The interaction order. Am. Sociol. Rev. 48, 1–17. doi: 10.2307/2095141

Goldin, C., and Katz, L. F. (2002). The power of the pill: oral contraceptives and women's career and marriage decisions. J. Polit. Econ. 110, 730–770. doi: 10.1086/340778

Golembiewski, R. T., Billingsley, K., and Yeager, S. (1976). Measuring change and persistence in human affairs: types of change generated by OD designs. J. Appl. Behav. Sci. 12, 133–157. doi: 10.1177/002188637601200201

Goodwin, R. (2006). Age and social support perception in Eastern Europe: social change and support in four rapidly changing countries. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 45, 799–815. doi: 10.1348/014466605X72144

Goodwin, R. B. (1998). Personal relationships and social change: the ‘Realpolitik’ of cross-cultural research in transient cultures. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 15, 227–247. doi: 10.1177/0265407598152006

Greenfield, P. M. (2009). Linking social change and developmental change: shifting pathways of human development. Dev. Psychol. 45, 401–418. doi: 10.1037/a0014726

Greenfield, P. M. (2016). Social change, cultural evolution, and human development. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 8, 84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.012

Habermas, H. (1987). The Theory of Communicative Action, 3rd Edn., Vol. 2 . Transl. by T. A. McCarthy. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.

Hannum, H. (1989). International law and Cambodian genocide: the sounds of silence. Hum. Rights Q. 11, 82–138. doi: 10.2307/761936

Hansen, N., Postmes, T., van der Vinne, N., and van Thiel, W. (2012). Information and communication technology and cultural change. Soc. Psychol. 43, 222–231. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000123

Harmon, D. J., Green, S. E., and Goodnight, G. T. (2015). A model of rhetorical legitimation: the structure of communication and cognition underlying institutional maintenance and change. Acad. Manag. Rev. 40, 76–95. doi: 10.5465/amr.2013.0310

Hegmon, M., Peeples, M. A., Kinzig, A. P., Kulow, S., Meegan, C. M., and Nelson, M. C. (2008). Social transformation and its human costs in the prehispanic U.S. Southwest. Am. Anthropol. 110, 313–324. doi: 10.1111/j.1548-1433.2008.00041.x

Herrera, C. (1999). Two arguments for covert methods in social research. Br. J. Sociol. 50, 331–343. doi: 10.1080/000713199358770

Hewitt, W. E., White, J., and Teevan, J. J. (2008). Introduction to Sociology: A Canadian Focus, 8th Edn . Toronto, ON: Pearson Prentice Hall.

Hinkle, G. J. (1952). The “four wishes” in Thomas' theory of social change. Soc. Res. 19, 464–484.

Hoffman, M. F., and Medlock-Klyukovski, A. (2004). “Our creator who art in heaven”: paradox, ritual, and cultural transformation. West. J. Commun. 68, 389–410. doi: 10.1080/10570310409374810

Homans, C. G. (1951). The Human Group . London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Jerneić, Ž., and Šverko, B. (2001). Life-role changes in times of socioeconomic transition. Rev. Psychol. 8, 41–48.

Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., and Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Polit. Psychol. 25, 881–919. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x

Kanter, R. M. (1991). Transcending business boundaries: 12,000 world managers view change. Harv. Bus. Rev. 69, 151–164.

Katz, D. (1974). Factors affecting social change: a social-psychological interpretation. J. Soc. Issues 30, 159–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1974.tb00732.x

Kelman, H. (1967). Humans use of human subjects: the problem of deception in social psychological experiments. Psychol. Bull. 67, 1–11. doi: 10.1037/h0024072

Kiesler, S., Siegel, J., and McGuire, T. W. (1984). Social psychological aspects of computer-mediated communication. Am. Psychol. 39, 1123–1134. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.39.10.1123

Kim, J. (2008). Perception of social change and psychological well-being: a study focusing on social change in Korea between 1997 and 2000. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 38, 2821–2858. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00415.x

Kim, J., and Ng, S. H. (2008). Perceptions of social changes and social identity: study focusing on Hong Kong society after reunification. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 11, 232–240. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2008.00262.x

Kirmayer, L. J., Dandeneau, S., Marshall, E., Phillips, M. K., and Williamson, K. J. (2011). Rethinking resilience from indigenous perspectives. Can. J. Psychiatry 56, 84–91. doi: 10.1177/070674371105600203

Kohn, M. L., Slomczynski, K. M., Janicka, K., Khmelko, V., Mach, B. W., Paniotto, V., et al. (1997). Social structure and personality under conditions of radical social change: a comparative analysis of Poland and Ukraine. Am. Sociol. Rev. 62, 614–638. doi: 10.2307/2657430

Kohn, M. L., Zaborowski, W., Janicka, K., Mach, B. W., Khmelko, V., Slomczynski, K. M., et al. (2000). Complexity of activities and personality under conditions of radical social change: a comparative analysis of Poland and Ukraine. Soc. Psychol. Q. 63, 187–207. doi: 10.2307/2695868

Kollontai, V. (1999). Social transformations in Russia. Int. Soc. Sci. J. 51, 103–121. doi: 10.1111/1468-2451.00180

Krznaric, R. (2007). How Change Happens: Interdisciplinary Perspectives for Human Development . Oxford: Oxfam.

Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., and Feldman, M. W. (2000). Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 131–175. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00002417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Landau, J., and Saul, J. (2004). “Facilitating family and community resilience in response to major disaster,” in Living Beyond Loss, 2nd Edn. , eds F. Walsh and M. McGoldrick (New York, NY: Norton), 285–309.

Lapuz, L. V. (1976). Culture change and psychological stress. Am. J. Psychoanal. 36, 171–176. doi: 10.1007/BF01248367

Latour, B. (2005). Reassembling the Social: An Introduction to Actor-Network-Theory . New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Lenski, G. E., and Lenski, J. (1974). Human Societies . New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Liu, H., Li, S., Xiao, Q., and Feldman, M. W. (2014). Social support and psychological well-being under social change in urban and rural China. Soc. Indic. Res. 119, 979–996. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0534-1

Liu, J. H., and Bernardo, A. B. I. (2014). Social psychology for social change: foundations for and introduction to a program of action-oriented research. J. Pacific Rim Psychol. 8, 29–34. doi: 10.1017/prp.2014.4

López, J., and Scott, J. (2000). Social Structure . Maidenhead: Open University Press.

PubMed Abstract

Luthar, S. S. (2003). Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities . New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Macek, P., Ježek, S., and Vazsonyi, A. T. (2013). Adolescents during and after times of social change: the case of the Czech Republic. J. Early Adolesc. 33, 1029–1047. doi: 10.1177/0272431613507758

Macionis, J. J., Jansson, M., and Benoit, C. M. (2008). Society: The Basics, 4th Edn. Toronto, ON: Pearson Education.

Marquis, D. G. (1947). Psychology of social change. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 249, 75–80. doi: 10.1177/000271624724900110

Marx, K. (1859/1970). A Contribution to the Critique of Political Economy . New York, NY: International Publishers.

Marx, K., and Engels, F. (1848). Manifesto of the Communist Party, Vol. 1 . Moscow: Progress Publishers.

Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: resilience processes in development. Am. Psychol. 56, 227–238. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.227

Masten, A. S., and Powell, J. L. (2003). “A resilience framework for research, policy, and practice,” in Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities , ed S. S. Luthar (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 1–25.

May, V. (2011). Self, belonging and social change. Sociology 45, 363–378. doi: 10.1177/0038038511399624

Mayhew, B. H. (1980). Structuralism versus individualism: part 1, shadowboxing in the dark. Soc. Forces 59, 335–375. doi: 10.2307/2578025

McDade, T. W., and Worthman, C. M. (2004). Socialization ambiguity in Samoan adolescents: a model for human development and stress in the context of culture change. J. Res. Adolesc. 14, 49–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2004.01401003.x

McGrath, J. E. (1983). Looking ahead by looking backwards: some recurrent themes about social change. J. Soc. Issues 39, 225–239. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1983.tb00186.x

Meyer, J., and Allen, N. (1997). Commitment in the Workplace: Theory, Research and Application . New York, NY: Sage publications.

Moghaddam, F. M. (2002). The Individual and Society: A Cultural Integration . New York, NY: Worth.

Moghaddam, F. M. (2012). The omnicultural imperative. Cult. Psychol. 18, 304–330. doi: 10.1177/1354067X12446230

Moghaddam, F. M., and Crystal, D. S. (1997). Revolutions, samurai, and reductions: the paradoxes of change and continuity in Iran and Japan. Polit. Psychol. 18, 355–384. doi: 10.1111/0162-895X.00061

Moghaddam, F. M., and Lvina, E. (2002). Toward a psychology of societal change and stability: the case of human rights and duties. Int. J. Group Tens. 31, 31–51. doi: 10.1023/A:1014212700468

Mucchi-Faina, A., Pacilli, M. G., and Pagliaro, S. (2010). Minority influence, social change, and social stability. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 4, 1111–1123. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00314.x

Nadler, D. A., and Tushman, M. L. (1995). “Types of organizational change: from incremental improvement to discontinuous transformation,” in Discontinuous Change: Leading Organizational Transformation , eds D. A. Nadler, R. B. Shaw, and A. E. Walton (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass), 14–33.

Neves, P., and Caetano, A. (2009). Commitment to change: contributions to trust in the supervisor and work outcomes. Group Organ. Manag. 34, 623–644. doi: 10.1177/1059601109350980

Newman, K. L. (2000). Organizational transformation during institutional upheaval. Acad. Manag. Rev. 25, 602–619.

Nisbet, R. A. (1972). Social Change . New York, NY: Harper and Row.

Noak, P., Kracke, B., Wild, E., and Hofer, M. (2001). Subjective experiences of social change in East and West Germany: analyses of the perceptions of adolescents and their parents. Am. Behav. Sci. 44, 1798–1817. doi: 10.1177/00027640121958168

Nolan, P., and Lenski, G. E. (2011). Human Societies: An Introduction to Macrosociology, 11th Edn . Boulder, CO: Paradigm.

Norris, F. H., Friedman, M. J., Watson, P. J., Byrne, C. M., Diaz, E., and Kaniasty, K. (2002). 60,000 disaster victims speak: part I. An empirical review of the empirical literature, 1981-2001. Psychiatry 65, 207–239. doi: 10.1521/psyc.65.3.207.20173

O'Keeffe, G. S., and Clarke-Pearson, K. (2011). The impact of social media on children, adult, and families. Am. Acad. Pediatr. 127, 800–806. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-0054

Oh, O., Eom, C., and Rao, H. R. (2015). Role of social media in social change: an analysis of collective sense making during the 2011 Egypt revolution. Inform. Syst. Res. 26, 210–223. doi: 10.1287/isre.2015.0565

Pareto, V. (1901/1968). The Rise and Fall of Elites: An Application of Theoretical Sociology . Towota, NJ: Bedminster Press.

Parson, T. (1951). The Social Structure of Social Action . New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Parsons, T. (1964). Essays in Sociological Theory . New York, NY: Free Press.

Pham, P. N., Weinstein, H. M., and Longman, T. (2004). Trauma and PTSD symptoms in Rwanda. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 292, 602–612. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.5.602

Pinquart, M., and Silbereisen, R. K. (2004). Human development in times of social change: theoretical considerations and research needs. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 28, 289–298. doi: 10.1080/01650250344000406

Pinquart, M., Silbereisen, R. K., and Juang, L. P. (2004). Moderating effects of adolescents' self-efficacy beliefs on psychological responses to social change. J. Adolesc. Res. 19, 340–359. doi: 10.1177/0743558403258851

Pinquart, M., Silbereisen, R. K., and Körner, A. (2009). Perceived work-related demands associated with social change, control strategies, and psychological well-being: do associations vary by regional economic conditions? Evidence from Germany. Eur. Psychol. 14, 207–219. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040.14.3.207

Pittenger, D. J. (2002). Deception in research: distinctions and solutions from the perspective of utilitarianism. Ethics Behav. 12, 117–142. doi: 10.1207/S15327019EB1202_1

Pizer, S. A., and Travers, J. R. (1975). Psychology and Social Change . New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Ponsioen, J. A. (1962). The Analysis of Social Change Reconsidered: A Sociological Study . The Hague: Mouton.

Porpora, D. V. (1989). Four concepts of social structure. J. Theory Soc. Behav. 19, 195–211. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5914.1989.tb00144.x

Prilleltensky, I. (1990). Enhancing the social ethics of psychology: toward a psychology at the service of social change. Can. Psychol. 31, 310–319. doi: 10.1037/h0078954

Prislin, R., and Christensen, P. N. (2005). Social change in the aftermath of successful minority influence. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 16, 43–73. doi: 10.1080/10463280440000071

Prunier, G. (2010). Africa's World War: Congo, the Rwandan Genocide, and the Making of a Continental Catastrophe . Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Reichers, A. E., John, P. W., and James, T. A. (1997). Understanding and managing cynicism about organizational change. Acad. Manag. Exec. 11, 48–59. doi: 10.5465/ame.1997.9707100659

Rieger, J. (2003). A retrospective visual study of social change: the pulp-logging industry in an Upper Peninsula Michigan county. Vis. Stud. 18, 157–178. doi: 10.1080/14725860310001632010

Rioufol, V. (2004). Approaches to social change in social forums: snapshots of recompositions in progress. Int. Soc. Sci. J. 56, 551–563. doi: 10.1111/j.0020-8701.2004.00516.x

Robinson, J. P., Barth, K., and Kohut, A. (1997). Social impact research: personal computers, mass media, and use of time. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 15, 65–82. doi: 10.1177/089443939701500107

Rocher, G. (1992). Introduction à la Sociologie Générale [General Introduction to Sociology] . Montréal, QC: Éditions Hurtubise.

Rodriguez, S. (2013). Making sense of social change: observing collective action in networked cultures. Sociol. Compass 7, 1053–1064. doi: 10.1111/soc4.12088

Rogers, E. M. (2003). Diffusion of Innovations, 5th Edn . New York, NY: Free Press.

Root Wolpe, P. (2006). Reasons scientists avoid thinking about ethics. Cell 125, 1023–1025. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.06.001

Round, J., and Williams, C. (2010). Coping with the social costs of “transition”: everyday life in post-Soviet Russia and Ukraine. Eur. Urban Reg. Stud. 17, 183–196. doi: 10.1177/0969776409356158

Rudel, T. K., and Hooper, L. (2005). Is the pace of social change accelerating? Latecomers, common languages, and rapid historical declines in fertility. Int. J. Comp. Sociol. 46, 275–296. doi: 10.1177/0020715205059204

Runciman, W. G. (1966). Relative Deprivation and Social Justice: A Study of Attitudes to Social Inequality in Twentieth-Century England . Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Sampson, E. E. (1989). The challenge of social change for psychology: globalization and psychology's theory of the person. Am. Psychol. 44, 914–921. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.6.914

Sanzgiri, J., and Gottlieb, J. Z. (1992). Philosophic and pragmatic influences on the practice of organization development, 1950-2000. Organiz. Dyn. 21, 57–69. doi: 10.1016/0090-2616(92)90064-T

Saran, A. K. (1963). The Marxian theory of social change. Inquiry 6, 70–128. doi: 10.1080/00201746308601368

Schaal, S., and Elbert, T. (2006). Ten years after the genocide: trauma confrontation and posttraumatic stress in Rwandan adolescents. J. Trauma Stress 19, 95–105. doi: 10.1002/jts.20104

Schneiderman, L. (1988). The Psychology of Social Change . Ann Arbor, MI: Human Sciences Press.

Schrickel, H. G. (1945). Group conflict and social reform. J. Soc. Psychol. 21, 187–196. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1945.9714165

Sewell, W. H. (1996). “Three temporalities: toward an eventful sociology,” in The Historic Turn in the Human Sciences , ed T. J. McDonald (Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press), 245–280.

Sidanius, J., and Pratto, F. (1999). Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Silbereisen, R. K., and Tomasik, M. J. (2010). Human behavior in response to social change: a guide to the special section. Eur. Psychol. 15, 243–245. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000059

Slone, M., Kaminer, D., and Durrheim, K. (2002). Appraisal of sociopolitical change among South African youth: the relation to psychological maladjustment. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 32, 318–341. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb00218.x

Sloutsky, V. M., and Searle-White, J. (1993). Psychological responses of Russians to rapid social change in the former USSR. Polit. Psychol. 14, 511–526. doi: 10.2307/3791710

Smelser, N. J., and Swedberg, R. (1994). “The sociological perspective on the economy,” in The Handbook of Economic Sociology , eds N. J. Smelser and R. Swedberg (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press), 3–26.

Smith, A. D. (1973). The Concept of Social Change: A Critique of the Functionalist Theory of Social Change . London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Spencer, H. (1898). The Principles of Sociology , Vol. 1. New York, NY: Appleton and Company.

Staub, E., Pearlman, L. A., Gubin, A., and Hagengimana, A. (2005). Healing, reconciliation, forgiving and the prevention of violence after genocide or mass killing: an intervention and its experimental evaluation in Rwanda. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 24, 297–334. doi: 10.1521/jscp.24.3.297.65617

Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., and Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: the psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 34, 379–440. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80009-0

Stinchcombe, A. L. (2000). “Social structure and organizations,” in Economics Meets Sociology in Strategic Management (Advances in Strategic Management) , Vol. 17, eds J. Baum and F. Dobbin (Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited), 229–259.

Stroebe, K., Wang, K., and Wright, S. C. (2015). Broadening perspectives on achieving social change. J. Soc. Issues 71, 633–645. doi: 10.1111/josi.12132

Subašić, E., Reynolds, K. J., and Turner, J. C. (2008). The political solidarity model of social change: dynamics of self-categorization in intergroup power relations. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 12, 330–352. doi: 10.1177/1088868308323223

Subašić, E., Reynolds, K. J., Reicher, S. D., and Klandermans, B. (2012). Where to from here for the psychology of social change? Future directions for theory and practice. Polit. Psychol. 33, 61–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00864.x

Sun, J., and Ryder, A. G. (2016). The Chinese experience of rapid modernization: sociocultural changes, psychological consequences? Front. Psychol. 7:477. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00477

Sztompka, P. (1993). The Sociology of Social Change . Oxford: Blackwell.

Sztompka, P. (1998). Devenir social, néo-modernisation et importance de la culture: quelques implications de la révolution anticommuniste pour la théorie du changement social [To become social, neo-modernization and importance of culture: a few implications of the anticommunist revolution for social change theory]. Sociol. Soc. 30, 85–94. doi: 10.7202/001781ar

Sztompka, P. (2000). Cultural trauma: the other face of social change. Eur. J. Soc. Theory 3, 449–466. doi: 10.1177/136843100003004004

Sztompka, P. (2004). “The trauma of social change: a case of postcommunist societies,” in Cultural Trauma and Collective Identity , eds J. C. Alexander, R. Eyerman, B. Giesen, N. J. Smelser, and P. Sztompka (Berkeley, CA: University of California Press), 155–195.

Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. C. (1986). “The social identity theory of intergroup behaviour,” in Psychology of Intergroup Relations , eds S. Worchel and W. G. Austin (Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall), 7–24.

Tanner, C. J., and Jackson, A. L. (2012). Social structure emerges via the interaction between local ecology and individual behaviour. J. Anim. Ecol. 81, 260–267. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2011.01879.x

Taylor, D. M. (1997). The quest for collective identity: the plight of disadvantaged ethnic minorities. Can. Psychol. 38, 174–190. doi: 10.1037/0708-5591.38.3.174

Taylor, D. M. (2002). The Quest for Identity: From Minority Groups to Generation Xers . Westport, CT: Greenwood.

Taylor, D. M., and de la Sablonnière, R. (2013). Why interventions in dysfunctional communities fail: the need for a truly collective approach. Can. Psychol. 54, 22–29. doi: 10.1037/a0031124

Taylor, D. M., and de la Sablonnière, R. (2014). Towards Constructive Change in Aboriginal Communities: A Social Psychology Perspective . Montréal, QC: McGill-Queen's Press.

Taylor, D. M., and McKirnan, D. J. (1984). Theoretical contributions: a five-stage model of intergroup relations. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 23, 291–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1984.tb00644.x

Terry, D. J., and Jimmieson, N. L. (2003). A stress and coping approach to organisational change: evidence from three field studies. Aust. Psychol. 38, 92–101. doi: 10.1080/00050060310001707097

Thomas, D. R., and Veno, A. (1992). Psychology and Social Change: Creating an International Agenda . Palmerston North: Dunmore Press.

Thompson, R. C., and Hunt, J. G. (1996). Inside the black box of alpha, beta, and gamma change: using a cognitive-processing model to assess attitude structure. Acad. Manag. Rev. 21, 655–690. doi: 10.2307/258998

Tilly, C., Tilly, L. A., and Tilly, R. H. (1975). The Rebellious Century: 1830-1930 . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Tomasik, M. J., Silbereisen, R. K., and Pinquart, M. (2010). Individuals negotiating demands of social and economic change: a control-theoretical approach. Eur. Psychol. 15, 246–259. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000064

Tonkens, E. (2012). Working with Arlie Hochschild: connecting feelings to social change. Soc. Polit. 19, 194–218. doi: 10.1093/sp/jxs003

Tushman, M. L., and Romanelli, E. (1985). Organizational evolution: a metamorphosis model of convergence and reorientation. Res. Organ. Behav. 7, 171–222.

Usborne, E., and de la Sablonnière, R. (2014). Understanding my culture means understanding myself: the function of cultural identity clarity for personal identity clarity and personal psychological well-being. J. Theory Soc. Behav. 44, 436–458. doi: 10.1111/jtsb.12061

Usborne, E., and Taylor, D. M. (2010). The role of cultural identity clarity for self-concept clarity, self-esteem, and subjective well-being. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36, 883–897. doi: 10.1177/0146167210372215

Vago, S. (2004). Social Change, 5th Edn . Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.

Van Binh, T. (2002). Social and cultural changes in Vietnam with the new market economy. Nat. Soc. Thought 15, 335–346.

Varnum, M. E. W. (2008). Rapid adaptation to social change in Central Europe: changes in locus of control, attribution, subjective well-being, self-direction, and trust. Sociológia 40, 215–235.

Vaughan, G. M. (1986). Social change and racial identity: issues in the use of picture and doll measures. Aust. J. Psychol. 38, 359–370. doi: 10.1080/00049538608259022

Veyssière, L. (2013). La Nouvelle-France en Héritage [New France in Heritage] . Paris: Armand Colin.

Walker, C., and Stephenson, S. (2010). Youth and social change in Eastern Europe and the former Soviet Union. J. Youth Stud. 13, 521–532. doi: 10.1080/13676261.2010.487522

Wall, K., and Louchakova, O. (2002). Evolution of consciousness in response to terrorist attacks: towards a transpersonal theory of cultural transformation. Humanist. Psychol. 30, 252–273. doi: 10.1080/08873267.2002.9977040

Watzlawick, P., Weakland, J. H., and Fisch, R. (1974). Change: Principles of Problem Formation and Problem Resolution . New York, NY: Norton.

Weinstein, J. (2010). Social Change, 3rd Edn . Lanham, MD: Roman and Littlefield.

Wilson, W. J. (2012). The Truly Disadvantaged: The Inner City, The Underclass, and Public Policy . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Wyn, J., and White, R. (2000). Negotiating social change: the paradox of youth. Youth Soc. 32, 165–183. doi: 10.1177/0044118X00032002002

Yakushko, O. (2008). The impact of social and political changes on survivors of political persecutions in rural Russia and Ukraine. Polit. Psychol. 29, 119–130. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2007.00615.x

Yanagizawa-Drott, D. (2014). Propaganda and conflict: evidence from the Rwandan genocide. Q. J. Econ. 129, 1947–1994. doi: 10.1093/qje/qju020

Zhang, X., and Hwang, S.-S. (2007). The micro consequences of macro-level social transition: how did Russians survive in the 1990s? Soc. Indic. Res. 82, 337–360. doi: 10.1007/s11205-006-9037-7

Zorbas, E. (2004). Reconciliation in post-genocide Rwanda. Afr. J. Legal Stud. 1, 29–52. doi: 10.1163/221097312X13397499735904

Zuck, A. M. (1997). Introduction. Am. Behav. Sci. 40, 257–258. doi: 10.1177/0002764297040003002

Keywords: identity, inertia, normative structure, social change, social structure, stability, pace of change, psychology of social change

Citation: de la Sablonnière R (2017) Toward a Psychology of Social Change: A Typology of Social Change. Front. Psychol. 8:397. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00397

Received: 25 August 2016; Accepted: 02 March 2017; Published: 28 March 2017.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2017 de la Sablonnière. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Roxane de la Sablonnière, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Module 18: Social Movements and Social Change

Causes of social change, learning outcomes.

  • Explain how technology, social institutions, population, and the environment can bring about social change

Collective behavior and social movements are just two of the forces driving social change , which is the change in society created through social movements as well as external factors like environmental shifts or technological innovations. Essentially, any disruptive shift in the status quo, be it intentional or random, human-caused or natural, can lead to social change. Below are some of the likely causes.

Changes to technology, social institutions, population, and the environment, alone or in some combination, create change. Below, we will discuss how these act as agents of social change, and we’ll examine real-world examples. We will focus on four agents of change that social scientists recognize: technology, social institutions, population, and the environment.

Some would say that improving technology has made our lives easier. Imagine what your day would be like without the Internet, the automobile, or electricity. In The World Is Flat , Thomas Friedman (2005) argues that technology is a driving force behind globalization, while the other forces of social change (social institutions, population, environment) play comparatively minor roles. He suggests that we can view globalization as occurring in three distinct periods. First, globalization was driven by military expansion, powered by horsepower and wind power. The countries best able to take advantage of these power sources expanded the most, and exert control over the politics of the globe from the late fifteenth century to around the year 1800. The second shorter period from approximately 1800 C.E. to 2000 C.E. consisted of a globalizing economy. Steam and rail power were the guiding forces of social change and globalization in this period. Finally, Friedman brings us to the post-millennial era. In this period of globalization, change is driven by technology, particularly the Internet (Friedman 2005).

But also consider that technology can create change in the other three forces social scientists link to social change. Advances in medical technology allow otherwise infertile women to bear children, which indirectly leads to an increase in population. Advances in agricultural technology have allowed us to genetically alter and patent food products, which changes our environment in innumerable ways. From the way we educate children in the classroom to the way we grow the food we eat, technology has impacted all aspects of modern life.

Of course there are drawbacks. The increasing gap between the technological haves and have-nots––sometimes called the digital divide––occurs both locally and globally. Further, there are added security risks: the loss of privacy, the risk of total system failure (like the Y2K panic at the turn of the millennium), and the added vulnerability created by technological dependence. Think about the technology that goes into keeping nuclear power plants running safely and securely. What happens if an earthquake or other disaster, like in the case of Japan’s Fukushima plant, causes the technology to malfunction, not to mention the possibility of a systematic attack to our nation’s relatively vulnerable technological infrastructure?

Technology and Crowdsourcing

Millions of people today walk around with their heads tilted toward a small device held in their hands. Perhaps you are reading this textbook on a phone or tablet. People in developed societies now take communication technology for granted. How has this technology affected social change in our society and others? One very positive way is crowdsourcing.

Thanks to the web, digital crowdsourcing is the process of obtaining needed services, ideas, or content by soliciting contributions from a large group of people, and especially from an online community rather than from traditional employees or suppliers. Web-based companies such as Kickstarter have been created precisely for the purposes of raising large amounts of money in a short period of time, notably by sidestepping the traditional financing process. This book, or virtual book, is the product of a kind of crowdsourcing effort. It has been written and reviewed by several authors in a variety of fields to give you free access to a large amount of data produced at a low cost. The largest example of crowdsourced data is Wikipedia, the online encyclopedia which is the result of thousands of volunteers adding and correcting material.

Perhaps the most striking use of crowdsourcing is disaster relief. By tracking tweets and e-mails and organizing the data in order of urgency and quantity, relief agencies can address the most urgent calls for help, such as for medical aid, food, shelter, or rescue. On January 12, 2010 a devastating earthquake hit the nation of Haiti. By January 25, a crisis map had been created from more than 2,500 incident reports, and more reports were added every day. The same technology was used to assist victims of the Japanese earthquake and tsunami in 2011, and many more times in disasters since then.

The Darker Side of Technology: Electronic Aggression in the Information Age

One dark side of technology is known as “electronic aggression”. The U.S. Center for Disease Control (CDC) uses the term to describe “any type of harassment or bullying that occurs through e-mail, a chat room, instant messaging, a website (including blogs), or text messaging” (CDC, n.d.). We generally think of this as cyberbullying. Cyberbullying represents a powerful change in modern society.  A 2011 study by the U.S. Department of Education found that 27.8 percent of students aged twelve through eighteen reported experiencing bullying. From the same sample 9 percent specifically reported having been a victim of cyberbullying (Robers et al. 2013). A more recent study conducted in 2016 on 5700 high school and middle school students between the ages of 12 and 17 found that 34% of students reported being cyber-bullied in their lifetime ( [1]

Cyberbullying is a special feature of the Internet. Something unique to electronic aggression is that it can happen twenty-four hours a day, every day—it can reach a child (or an adult) even though she or he might otherwise feel safe in a locked house. The messages and images may be posted anonymously and to a very wide audience, and they might even be impossible to trace. Finally, once posted, the texts and images are very hard to delete. Its effects range from the use of alcohol and drugs to lower self-esteem, health problems, and even suicide (CDC, n.d.).

The Story of Megan Meier

According to the Megan Meier Foundation web site (2014a), Megan Meier had a lifelong struggle with weight, attention deficit disorder, and depression. But then a sixteen-year-old boy named Josh Evans asked Megan, who was thirteen years old, to be friends on the social networking web site MySpace. The two began communicating online regularly, though they never met in person or spoke on the phone. Now Megan finally knew a boy who, she believed, really thought she was pretty.

But things changed, according to the Megan Meier Foundation web site (2014b). Josh began saying he didn’t want to be friends anymore, and the messages became cruel on October 16, 2006, when Josh concluded by telling Megan, “The world would be a better place without you.” The cyberbullying escalated when additional classmates and friends on MySpace began writing disturbing messages and bulletins. That night Megan hanged herself in her bedroom closet, three weeks before what would have been her fourteenth birthday.

According to an ABC News article titled, “Parents: Cyber Bullying Led to Teen’s Death” (2007), it was only later that a neighbor informed Megan’s parents that Josh was not a real person. Instead, “Josh’s” account was created by the mother of a girl who used to be friends with Megan.

You can find out more of Megan’s story at her mother’s web site .

or through her Ted Talk, “What Kids Have To Say About Bullying And How To End It” .

Social Institutions

Each change in a single social institution leads to changes in all social institutions. For example, the industrialization of society meant that there was no longer a need for large families to produce enough manual labor to run a farm. Further, new job opportunities were in close proximity to urban centers where living space was at a premium. The result is that the average family size shrunk significantly.

This same shift toward industrial corporate entities also changed the way we view government involvement in the private sector, created the global economy, provided new political platforms, and even spurred new religions and new forms of religious worship like Scientology. It has also informed the way we educate our children: originally schools were set up to accommodate an agricultural calendar so children could be home to work the fields in the summer, and even today, teaching models are largely based on preparing students for industrial jobs, despite that being an outdated need. A shift in one area, such as industrialization, means an interconnected impact across social institutions.

Population composition is changing at every level of society. Births increase in one nation and decrease in another. Some families delay childbirth while others start bringing children into their folds early. Population changes can be due to random external forces, like an epidemic, or shifts in other social institutions, as described above. But regardless of why and how it happens, population trends have a tremendous interrelated impact on all other aspects of society. For example, because we are experiencing an increase in our senior population as baby boomers begin to retire, this will in turn change the way many of our social institutions are organized. For instance, there is an increased demand for housing in warmer climates, a massive shift in the need for elder care and assisted living facilities, and growing awareness of elder abuse. There is concern about labor shortages as boomers retire, not to mention the knowledge gap as the most senior and accomplished leaders in different sectors start to leave. Further, as this large generation leaves the workforce, the loss of tax income and pressure on pension and retirement plans means that the financial stability of the country is threatened.

Globally, often the countries with the highest fertility rates are least able to absorb and attend to the needs of a growing population. Family planning is a large step in ensuring that families are not burdened with more children than they can care for. On a macro level, the increased population, particularly in the poorest parts of the globe, also leads to increased stress on the planet’s resources.

The Environment

Turning to human ecology, we know that individuals and the environment affect each other. As human populations move into more vulnerable areas, we see an increase in the number of people affected by natural disasters, and we see that human interaction with the environment increases the impact of those disasters. Part of this is simply the numbers: the more people there are on the planet, the more likely it is that some will be affected by a natural disaster.

But it goes beyond that. Movements like 350.org describe how we have already seen five extinctions of massive amounts of life on the planet, and the crisis of global change has put us on the verge of yet another. According to their website, “The number 350 means climate safety: to preserve a livable planet, scientists tell us we must reduce the amount of CO2 in the atmosphere from its current level of 400 parts per million to below 350 ppm” (350.org).

The environment is best described as an ecosystem, one that exists as the interplay of multiple parts including 8.7 million species of life. However dozens of species are going extinct every day, a number 1,000 times to 10,000 times the normal “background rate” and the highest rate since the dinosaurs disappeared 65 million years ago. The Center for Biological Diversity states that this extinction crisis, unlike previous ones caused by natural disasters, is “caused almost entirely by us” (Center for Biological Diversity, n.d.). The growth of the human population, currently over seven billion and expected to rise to nine or ten billion by 2050, perfectly correlates with the rising extinction rate of life on earth.

Hurricane Katrina: When It All Comes Together

We’ve mentioned that four key elements that affect social change are the environment , technology , social institutions , and population . In 2005, New Orleans was struck by a devastating hurricane. But it was not just the hurricane that was disastrous. It was the converging of all four of these elements, and the text below will connect the elements by putting the words in parentheses.

Before Hurricane Katrina (environment) hit, poorly coordinated evacuation efforts had left about 25 percent of the population, almost entirely African Americans who lacked private transportation, to suffer the consequences of the coming storm (demographics). Then “after the storm, when the levees broke, thousands more [refugees] came. And the city buses, meant to take them to proper shelters, were underwater” (Sullivan 2005). No public transportation was provided, drinking water and communications were delayed, and FEMA, the Federal Emergency Management Agency (institutions), was headed by an appointee with no real experience in emergency management. Those who were eventually evacuated did not know where they were being sent or how to contact family members. African Americans were sent the farthest from their homes. When the displaced began to return, public housing had not been reestablished, yet the Superdome stadium, which had served as a temporary disaster shelter, had been rebuilt. Homeowners received financial support, but renters did not.

As it turns out, it was not entirely the hurricane that cost the lives of 1,500 people, but the fact that the city’s storm levees (technology), which had been built too low and which failed to meet numerous other safety specifications, gave way, flooding the lower portions of the city, occupied almost entirely by African Americans.

Journalist Naomi Klein, in her book The Shock Doctrine: The Rise of Disaster Capitalism, presents a theory of a “triple shock,” consisting of an initial disaster, an economic shock that replaces public services with private (for-profit) ones, and a third shock consisting of the intense policing of the remaining public. Klein supports her claim by quoting then-Congressman Richard Baker as saying, “We finally cleaned up public housing in New Orleans. We couldn’t do it, but God did.” She quotes developer Joseph Canizaro as stating, “I think we have a clean sheet to start again. And with that clean sheet we have some very big opportunities.”

One clean sheet opportunity was that New Orleans began to replace public schools with charters, breaking the teachers’ union and firing all public school teachers (Mullins 2014). Public housing was seriously reduced and the poor were forced out altogether or into the suburbs far from medical and other facilities (The Advocate 2013). Finally, by relocating African Americans and changing the ratio of African Americans to whites, New Orleans changed its entire demographic makeup.

Think It Over

  • Consider one of the major social movements of the twentieth century, from civil rights in the United States to Gandhi’s nonviolent protests in India. How would technology have changed it? Would change have come more quickly or more slowly? Defend your opinion.
  • Discuss the digital divide in the context of modernization. Is there a real concern that poorer communities are lacking in technology? Why, or why not?
  • Which theory do you think better explains the global economy: dependency theory (global inequity is due to the exploitation of peripheral and semi-peripheral nations by core nations) or modernization theory? Remember to justify your answer and provide specific examples.
  • Do you think that modernization is beneficial or detrimental to societies? Explain, using examples.
  • https://cyberbullying.org/2016-cyberbullying-data ↵
  • Modification, adaptation, and original content. Authored by : Rebecca Vonderhaar for Lumen Learning. Provided by : Lumen Learning. License : CC BY: Attribution
  • Social Change. Authored by : OpenStax CNX. Located at : https://cnx.org/contents/[email protected]:vi4eB2eh@5/Social-Change . License : CC BY: Attribution . License Terms : Download for free at http://cnx.org/contents/[email protected]

Footer Logo Lumen Waymaker

Social Change: Definition, Characteristics, Causes, Types, and Examples

Humans are social beings. We exist in a social world and observe norms, rules and traditions that are all social constructs. Therefore social change is a concept that is threaded to the very root of society. Similar to the Earth completing a strenuous rotation around the Sun every year, society undergoes social change. It shapes and shifts how we perceive the world and its social interactions. Comparing society from the 19th century to the current 21st will present itself with drastic changes. For starters, women’s rights was a concept a majority of men refused to believe in and blatant racism towards people of colour was an accepted part of one’s daily routine. Social change is what has brought about so many alterations to the functioning of society.

What does Social Change mean?

According to M. E Jones, social change occurs when social processes, social patterns, social interactions or social organisations encounter modifications from their everyday functioning. Marx, on the other hand, looked at social change as the consequence of class struggle- the conflict was the primary means for change. He believes that the economy is the most significant causing factor for change. Many sociologists have correlated the relationship between technology and social change. In earlier years, societies were largely nomadic and depended on manual human labour to hunt and gather for food on a daily basis. As humans evolved and built contraptions that eased the process of cultivation and agricultural production, societies began to claim land and mark territories. Lenski believed that these technological advancements are what caused societies to shift, and it is possible to observe how technological advancements affect the very thread of society to this day.

Theories of Change

Characteristics of Social Change

            Idrani (1998) and Anele (1999) observed and attributed certain characteristics to social change.

Advancements in technology also led to the improvement of the medical and agricultural world, both of which have multitudes of benefits. High-tech medical equipment now makes it easier for doctors to treat critically ill patients and have also eased the process of childbirth,  greatly reducing the number of deaths during childbirth. The development of large agricultural fields and equipment have made it easier to grow to produce in bulk for commercial purposes. Food scientists are also finding ways to create hybrid fruits and vegetables which are packed with nutrients. The impact of technology extends towards culture as well. Different cultures come into contact with and influence each other. Globalisation is a key example of how technology has brought about cultural change.

However, it is important to observe how technology has created a digital divide and is furthering the inequalities faced by marginalised and underprivileged sections of society. Those with access to the internet and digital resources can arm themselves with information while the rest are left to their own devices or a lack of one.

Natural disasters can uproot societies and it takes a while to build them back to what it was before. These disasters also highlight the importance of environmental activism and urge individuals to show more care and support to the land they live in. Activists such as Greta Thunberg have immensely contributed to creating awareness of environmentalism. It has put large organisations and industries under the spotlight and expose how the present way of functioning is causing immense damage to the earth. It has also led to smaller changes such as switching from plastic bags to cloth bags and plastic straws to metal ones.

Social conflict is perhaps the most easily observable cause for social change and usually results in large-scale protests and public demonstrations. With the help of the internet, social change exists and can be executed in the digital world as well. Social conflict can occur due to class-based struggles, gender issues, racism and ethnic discrimination. It all highlights how the majority is in dominance and exploits and harasses the marginalised. Wars are major events that are discussed when talking about social conflicts in the past.

Types and examples of social change

            Revolutionary change, on the other hand, occurs like rapid-fire and its results can be viewed over a short period of time. These kinds of changes are more present today in the digital world because of how accessible news and platforms to share information are. The Arab Spring is an example of revolutionary change. It began in 2010 and the movement lasted till 2013. The Arab Spring is a series of anti-government protests, uprisings and demonstrations that fought and challenged authoritarian regimes. It began in Tunisia and spread to almost every Arab nation and it can be argued to have caused the most significant change in the middle east since it was decolonised. The #MeToo movement is another social movement that brought about a revolutionary change as it led to bills and laws being passed to be more stringent about the persecution of harassers on-site. Similarly, the BLM caused an international uproar and demanded those who were guilty of committing such treacherous acts of racism be punished.

            Change can manifest as a positive or negative reaction depending on the context. As mentioned earlier, technological advancements have brought about immense development but at the same time, has created a vast digital divide. It is important to look at social change from a holistic perspective and understand how social change can occur at a micro-and macro-level simultaneously. Change is inevitable so it would be futile to halt the process. Moreover, with how fast-paced the 21st century is, change is required. This ensures that social institutions and structures are kept in check and help with the growth of society and all its people.

How Participatory Action Research Can Promote Social Change and Help Youth Development

  • December 2012
  • SSRN Electronic Journal

Cara Berg Powers at Clark University

  • Clark University

Erin Allaman at Colorado Mountain College

  • Colorado Mountain College

Discover the world's research

  • 25+ million members
  • 160+ million publication pages
  • 2.3+ billion citations

Tara D. Hudson

  • Darris R. Means
  • Elizabeth Tish

Deborah H. John

  • Beret Halverson
  • Child Adolesc Soc Work J
  • Nneka A. Arinze

Jennifer McGarry

  • Najwan Hani Mahmoud
  • Roshaunda L. Breeden
  • Tavaria L. Smith
  • Aniya L. Willis

Marisa MacDonnell

  • Angela Wang

Maurice J. Elias

  • Kavitha Mediratta

Jessica Ruglis

  • Brett G. Stoudt
  • J M R Duncan-Andrade
  • S Ginwright
  • Recruit researchers
  • Join for free
  • Login Email Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google Welcome back! Please log in. Email · Hint Tip: Most researchers use their institutional email address as their ResearchGate login Password Forgot password? Keep me logged in Log in or Continue with Google No account? Sign up

Stanford Social Innovation Review Logo

  • Arts & Culture
  • Civic Engagement
  • Economic Development
  • Environment
  • Human Rights
  • Social Services
  • Water & Sanitation
  • Foundations
  • Nonprofits & NGOs
  • Social Enterprise
  • Collaboration
  • Design Thinking
  • Impact Investing
  • Measurement & Evaluation
  • Organizational Development
  • Philanthropy & Funding
  • Current Issue
  • Sponsored Supplements
  • Global Editions
  • In-Depth Series
  • Stanford PACS
  • Submission Guidelines

Addressing Trauma as a Pathway to Social Change

How understanding intergenerational trauma can help people working toward social change solve problems more effectively.

  • order reprints
  • related stories

By Ijeoma Njaka & Duncan Peacock Jan. 21, 2021

how can a research project cause social change

In the 1860s, American abolitionist Frederick Douglass noted that people invested in social change “ endeavor to remove the contradiction ” between “what ought to be” and “what is.” This contradiction seems ubiquitous today as social change advocates struggle to address multiple, overlapping crises. Systemic racism, climate change , the forced displacement of millions of people , a devastating global pandemic, and other widespread social issues highlight how far we are from “what ought to be.” And each of these problems requires urgent and sustained attention.

At the same time, another problem is inhibiting and limiting sustained attention to these complex crises: trauma. We are learning that trauma, or distress resulting from exposure to chronic or extreme mental or physical stress, is a common human experience with the power to spread across people and time. Research reveals that if nothing is done to mitigate the influence of traumatic experiences, individuals afflicted by today’s challenges may pass their trauma to the next generation. This kind of transfer, known as “intergenerational trauma,” isn’t new to human experience, but research on it didn’t begin in earnest until the 1960s. Currently, the American Psychological Association describes it as “a phenomenon in which the descendants of a person who has experienced a terrifying event show adverse emotions and behavioral reactions to the event that are similar to those of the person him/herself.” Intergenerational trauma can persist long after the memory of the initial traumatic event has faded.

Centered Self: The Connection Between Inner Well-Being and Social Change

Related concepts about the way sources, experiences, or effects of trauma include but are not limited to individuals also sheds light on the problem. Collective trauma, for example, is a shared traumatic experience that can negatively affect entire communities. The “demoralization, disorientation, and loss of connection” of survivors at the Buffalo Creek disaster of 1972 is just one example. It can also refer to mass suffering incurred by the kind of high death rates and economic and social shutdowns we’ve seen during the COVID-19 pandemic. While researchers and others make appropriate distinctions between terms such as collective, historical , cultural , multigenerational, transgenerational , and generational trauma based on context, each expresses the phenomena of inherited or shared trauma.

As previous articles in this series have discussed, change makers—those actively engaged in building a healthy and just society—not only face and address trauma among the populations they serve, but also commonly experience personal harm and trauma as a result of their work. There is also growing evidence that these experiences impair their ability to foster organizational and social change, and that supporting personal well-being lies at the heart of effectively addressing social challenges. Until we better recognize the impact trauma has on individuals, organizations, and society, those working for social change will be limited in their ability to support human dignity, well-being, and resilience. Indeed, we must better understand trauma, discuss it, and integrate awareness of it into the culture of social change if we are to effectively address issues in which trauma and intergenerational trauma are factors.

With this in mind, a think tank launched by The Wellbeing Project and Georgetown University recently started a research program to better understand the connection between inner work (including self-inquiry, reflection, and self-care), outer change (work that individuals do to effect change in the world), and intergenerational trauma. The program is exploring intergenerational trauma with the idea that understanding its impact might advance the well-being of individuals working toward social change and the communities they work with, as well as increase the impact of their work. An initial field scan and literature review conducted in 2020 has revealed a diverse range of perspectives on intergenerational trauma through human stories and a variety of research-based disciplinary approaches. Here’s a look at what we’ve learned so far.

Defining the Challenge

Intergenerational trauma was initially documented in 1960s and 1970s studies of the Holocaust. Helen Epstein’s article “ Children of the Holocaust ” investigates how the experience and wounds of Holocaust survivors effected the lives of the following generation. Over time, the field has expanded to examine intergenerational trauma as a result of war, genocide, refugee experiences , health disparities , apartheid in South Africa , India’s caste system , American slavery and its legacies, poverty , and the oppression and violence against Indigenous Peoples . These and similar phenomena have been the source of mass trauma that has directly or indirectly affected multiple generations. However, intergenerational trauma doesn’t necessarily result from widespread social calamity. It can also result from unresolved trauma endured by mothers, or from trauma experienced by parents, grandparents, and further descendants, who can, knowingly or unknowingly, pass it down through feelings, memories, and even language .

Defining the problem isn’t just about identifying historical or ongoing instances of violence that cause this sort of trauma; it’s also about using the questions, methods, and tools of different fields and disciplines, as well as human stories, to examine and ultimately heal the damage at hand. A range of documented cases that incorporate epigenetics , or the study of heritable changes in gene function that aren’t part of the genetic code, show that stressors and trauma can change gene expression, and thus pass between generations . Historical trauma, such as trauma resulting from the forced displacement and destruction of Native American communities, has deleterious effects on physiological, psychological, and social dimensions of generations of lives. In addition to biology, this complex challenge is therefore also a concern of psychology, economics, and education. Given the relationship between social media, mental health, and democracy, the spread of trauma is also a concern of technology and ethics .

We are all situated in relation to historical and cultural narratives. If marked by trauma, the narratives people tell themselves and concepts of self can be maladaptive and can become mechanisms of historical trauma . The arts and storytelling are necessary tools for creating healthier narratives, and for creating narratives that honor the past but enable healing in the present and future. Going forward, researchers and practitioners must use a wide spectrum of interdisciplinary approaches informed by cross-sector and global perspectives to explore and document this topic.

Intergenerational Trauma as a Lens for Practicing Social Change

Over the past year, we’ve gleaned five initial insights about the connection between intergenerational trauma and the social change sector. In addition to undergirding our future research, we hope these insights can offer thought-provoking framing for current social change leaders.

1. Finding root causes and compounding factors . Recognizing intergenerational trauma can help spotlight and define the roots of social change issues.

Consider the high costs of poverty in the United States . Getting to the roots of the problem includes recognizing that the problem is not simply about individual failures and shortcomings. Addressing poverty requires that we ask questions about underlying systemic factors, such as structural racism , which compounds impoverishment by the mechanisms of discrimination and racial health disparities. It also raises questions about the unequal distribution of power and wealth , which affects economic, social, and political agency, including mobility in any of these areas.

Poverty also creates major challenges to mental well-being, not only due to lack of access to vital resources, but also because the chronic stress of living through poverty can lead to atrophy of the prefrontal cortex , an area of the brain needed for higher reasoning and control, judgement, and decision making. In other words, trauma caused by poverty itself can become a compounding factor that makes it more difficult to exit the cycle of poverty.

By looking at root causes and compounding circumstances, social change makers can deepen their understanding of the challenges they wish to solve and, in turn, shape more effective, future interventions.

2. Supporting change makers and the communities they serve . Addressing intergenerational trauma can help support change makers who have experienced intergenerational trauma themselves, including those who have chosen to work in social change partly to address the source of their trauma.

During the Rhodes Must Fall movement at the University of Cape Town (UCT), for example, student activists demanded the removal of a statue of imperialist Cecil Rhodes, who implemented policies that enabled the institutionalization of apartheid. Although many of the students were born after apartheid formally ended, the legacy of apartheid and colonial trauma remained . In 2016, the students created The Fall , a play about two UCT movements: Rhodes Must Fall and Fees Must Fall. These activist-artists found the process of creating the play and sharing their experiences therapeutic and healing for both themselves and their audience.

This type of process, which links the inner work of confronting intergenerational trauma with social change focused on its effects, is an effective way to both enhance well-being for change makers and broaden the reach of social change.

3. Recognizing trauma in the interconnectedness of the world . We are also learning that there are porous boundaries between the social problems we face today and the collective trauma that exacerbates them. In some instances, foundations of trauma cascade across seemingly unrelated areas of life and society.

Road congestion and gridlock in Atlanta, Georgia, for example , have roots in its interstate’s design to accommodate racial segregation while prioritizing white neighborhoods. They also reflect resistance to expanded public transit access from predominantly white areas. More broadly, capitalistic mechanisms that lead to increased economic mobility for certain populations (for example, educated or corporate groups) have negative impacts on personal health and families and the futures of low-income children .

For social change leaders, examining interconnectivity adds nuance to the social problems they work on and expands the possible types of resources that may address these issues. The interconnectedness of the world necessitates the use of many disciplines and human perspectives in concert with one another.

4. Activating healing from trauma . In his book, “The Body Keeps the Score,” trauma expert Bessel Van der Kolk explains that trauma compromises a person’s agency and ability to cope with overwhelming situations, implicating the well-being of the body, brain, and mind. However, it’s important to note that factors such as social support can mitigate the degree to which trauma takes hold in any given individual, and how it impacts even those who experienced the same event or conditions . And while trauma has the pernicious ability to become a vicious cycle, the pursuit of agency, dignity, and well-being can create the reverse—a virtuous cycle of healing.

The story of Desmond Meade, president of the Florida Rights Restoration Coalition, provides an example on the individual level. Voting for the first time in 2020 motivated him more than ever to continue restoring millions of other people’s right to vote. The agency he experienced when voting helped him reconnect with a sense of dignity—his inherent value as a participant in the democratic process. It also helped his psychological well-being, particularly as he considered the challenges of both his life and the lives of his historically disenfranchised Black American ancestors.

Another example is the therapeutic storytelling practice Testimony Therapy , which has helped survivors of war and political violence, and political refugees heal from trauma by sharing their personal stories in a way that rigorously documents and preserves their testimony.

Social change initiatives of all kinds can also incorporate measures to heal trauma. For example, restorative-justice initiatives such as the Truth and Reconciliation Commission in South Africa and the Colombia Peace Process both included reparation and rehabilitation measures, with the aim of giving formerly oppressed people a voice and fostering agency. And from their inception in the United States, both the civil rights movement of the 1950s and 1960s , and the current Black Lives Matter movement have asserted the dignity of all, and demanded political, economic, and social agency for those who have been oppressed and excluded.

These examples illustrate how social change leaders can develop their own initiatives and movements in ways that use agency, dignity, and well-being as methods for addressing cycles of trauma.

5. Honoring the role of time and connection . When American civil rights leader Rev. Dr. Martin Luther King Jr. emphasized the maxim , “Justice too long delayed is justice denied,” he recognized the role of time in achieving justice. In a similar way, healing that is too long delayed not only is denied, but also can allow problems to worsen. The longer people experience traumatizing conditions or unresolved trauma, the more trauma can compound , and the more it can fatigue people , even spreading to others who otherwise may have remained resilient or whose particular experiences were radically different.

Scholar Suraj Yengde has described the traumas he and other generations of Dalits have endured from the caste system. In addition to shared conditions of diminished mobility, poverty, abuse , and cultural erasure , Yengde notes how trauma can even limit the dreams and aspirations other community members. Time is therefore of the essence when it comes to healing. And just as unaddressed trauma can permeate through generations over time , addressing trauma requires sustained attention and support .

Connection—between body to mind, one human to another, one generation to the next, and inner selves to outer environments—is another distinctive element of healing. Interoception (awareness of what’s going on in the body), somatic therapy (grounding in the body through movement), and relationships and a sense of belonging all facilitate healing by re-establishing connections that are fundamental for humans.

Writer and founder of the Academy of Inner Science Thomas Hübl suggests that one of the most important connections for healing collective and intergenerational trauma is between science and spirit, which he argues brings together the “double helix of ancient wisdom and contemporary understanding.” Humans experience well-being when we have agency, dignity, and health, and are connected to ourselves, each other, and our world in sustainable and life-giving ways. Trauma is the disconnection from these things. Those working on social change, therefore, need to identify the connections and disconnections in the issues they care about. They also need support when they experience disconnection in their own lives.

A Call to Action

The social sector needs to take a few steps to address intergenerational trauma, and achieve the inner well-being of individuals and communities. First, we must recognize intergenerational trauma as a widespread challenge to achieving well-being at scale—one that requires ongoing attention and healing in different forms for different human experiences. Second, we need to advocate for the active exploration of intergenerational trauma within the social change sector—indeed, for it to become a core focus, integrated with mainstream work. Third, we must deepen scholarship on intergenerational trauma using an interdisciplinary approach informed by a range of diverse human experiences and stories. And finally, we must develop methods and practices for healing that join inner and outer work, and connect dignity, agency, and well-being to avenues for social change.

Over the next year, The Wellbeing Project and Georgetown University—in conjunction with a global, multidisciplinary panel of experts and associate voices—will continue to explore the effects of intergenerational trauma on individuals, communities, and the social sector, with the following principles in mind:

1. Examine the roots, not just the symptoms of these problems. Instances of intergenerational trauma can often be broken down into a multitude of interrelated factors.

2. Synthesize a range of perspectives and connect them in new ways. This includes using many disciplines, examining many sectors of human life, and listening to many human stories to understand how intergenerational trauma begins and unfolds.

3. Establish a range of approaches to healing, including using varied forms of therapy or well-being practices , using the arts and storytelling, bridging cultural divides, advocating , researching , enacting policy, and changing laws .

4. Engage and contribute to the work of various sectors, including social change, policy, and education.

In a November 2020 interview , former US President Barack Obama said that the country will continue to fall short in the change Americans seek if we only address symptoms and ignore the underlying causes of social problems. He described how the deaths of too many Black Americans “is part and parcel with a legacy of discrimination, and Jim Crow, and segregation,” and that the only way to address the injustices of the criminal justice system is to look at the economy, housing, and other interconnected factors that impede progress. “The good news,” he said, “is that we can all take responsibility.” These insights also apply to social change leaders addressing intergenerational trauma and well-being. We can begin to address traumatic experiences that have permeated communities through identifying root causes, supporting change makers’ healing, and understanding interconnected problems and outcomes. The ubiquity of these obstacles means that the opportunity for change is ubiquitous as well.

Support  SSIR ’s coverage of cross-sector solutions to global challenges.  Help us further the reach of innovative ideas.  Donate today .

Read more stories by Ijeoma Njaka & Duncan Peacock .

SSIR.org and/or its third-party tools use cookies, which are necessary to its functioning and to our better understanding of user needs. By closing this banner, scrolling this page, clicking a link or continuing to otherwise browse this site, you agree to the use of cookies.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

The PMC website is updating on October 15, 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Front Psychol

Toward a Psychology of Social Change: A Typology of Social Change

Millions of people worldwide are affected by dramatic social change (DSC). While sociological theory aims to understand its precipitants, the psychological consequences remain poorly understood. A large-scale literature review pointed to the desperate need for a typology of social change that might guide theory and research toward a better understanding of the psychology of social change. Over 5,000 abstracts from peer-reviewed articles were assessed from sociological and psychological publications. Based on stringent inclusion criteria, a final 325 articles were used to construct a novel, multi-level typology designed to conceptualize and categorize social change in terms of its psychological threat to psychological well-being. The typology of social change includes four social contexts: Stability, Inertia, Incremental Social Change and, finally, DSC. Four characteristics of DSC were further identified: the pace of social change, rupture to the social structure, rupture to the normative structure, and the level of threat to one's cultural identity. A theoretical model that links the characteristics of social change together and with the social contexts is also suggested. The typology of social change as well as our theoretical proposition may serve as a foundation for future investigations and increase our understanding of the psychologically adaptive mechanisms used in the wake of DSC.

“Change— extremely rapid social change —is the most important fact of life today” (Nolan and Lenski, 2011 , p. xiii).

Zoia is a lively 75-year-old Baboushka . Her eventful life has seen her experience some less-than-welcome adventures, but she has always managed to adapt to unfamiliar circumstances. After completing her studies in Moscow, she was, like many other young educated Russians, deported by USSR authorities to another state. Her destination was Frunze (later renamed Bishkek), a land in Central Asia warmer than hers and made slightly cooler by its unfamiliarity. Despite the diversity of Frunze, with ethnic Kyrgyz, Ukrainians, and other Slavic groups forming sizeable minorities, the Russian population remained a majority. During the Soviet era, Zoia was told that she lived in one of the most powerful countries in the world, where crime rates were low and the population enjoyed decent education and food supply, as well as the opportunity to save money for retirement.

The diversity of ethnicities eventually bred great tension, and the collapse of the Soviet Union in the early 1990s deeply affected Zoia's life. At the age of 54, she learned that her country was in ruins, that her rights as a Russian were diminished and that her language was widely frowned upon within the newly formed Kyrgyz Republic, Kyrgyzstan. Meanwhile, the disorganized authority allowed for an explosion in crime rates and increasing scarcity of resources. Zoia lost all of her life savings. The money she earned was no longer sufficient to cover basic necessities. Despite her position as a chief engineer, Zoia was forced to work a second job selling newspapers at the corner of her street just to make ends meet.

Although Zoia's story may seem uniquely dramatic, it is only one among over one billion (Sun and Ryder, 2016 ). Social change is indiscriminately pervasive and global—restricted to neither developing nor western worlds (e.g., Ponsioen, 1962 ; Smith, 1973 ; Chirot and Merton, 1986 ; Zuck, 1997 ; Sztompka, 1998 ; Fukuyama, 1999 ; Weinstein, 2010 ; Nolan and Lenski, 2011 ; Greenfield, 2016 ). Dramatic social change (DSC) is the new normal and can be witnessed presently across a multitude of contexts from political and economic upheaval, to desperate mass migration, and from natural or human disasters to technological advances.

Social change has always been a field of great interest for the social sciences, especially among sociologists since it seems that “all sociology is about change” (Sztompka, 1993 , p.xiii; see also Sztompka, 2004 ). Many sociology texts have entire sections devoted to social change (e.g., Bauman, 2003 ; Latour, 2005 ; Hewitt et al., 2008 ; Giddens et al., 2011 ) all aimed at addressing one main question: What leads to social change ? Many sociological theories have been suggested to explain the different “macro” processes associated with the onset of revolutions, social movements, or important technological changes. A “macro” theory focuses on the structural factors or defining events that contribute to DSC and are useful when considering how social changes are brought upon an entire group, community, institution, nation, or indeed society as a whole. The macro approach, however, is seriously limited when it comes to “micro” processes, which focus on the equally important question of the consequences of social change, or, in other words, how individual group members are impacted by social change (e.g., Rogers, 2003 ). Thus, the exclusive research focus on macro processes has left unanswered the pivotal question: What are the psychological consequences of social change?

Given the potentially dire consequences of DSC, it is surprising that psychologists have neglected it as a topic of rigorous academic pursuit, particularly given the current reality of vast globalization and massive immigration. To date, research focusing on the impact of social change on the well-being of individuals has not been clearly established (Kim, 2008 ; Liu et al., 2014 ). Moreover, the adaptation mechanisms that people develop when coping with such contexts remain largely unknown (Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004 ).

The goal of the present paper is to argue that psychology needs to focus on the psychology of social change (de la Sablonnière et al., 2013 ; de la Sablonnière and Usborne, 2014 ). I argue that the bridge between the “macro” processes of social change and the “micro” processes of its psychological impacts have yet to be built. I suggest that social scientists must first focus on conceptualizing social change in a manner that includes both macro and micro processes in order to understand individuals' adaptation to social change. Thus, as the first step in moving toward a psychology of social change, I target what is considered the most difficult challenge: conceptualizing social change.

First and foremost, conceptualizing social change requires untangling the complexity of the topic by formulating a typology of social change (see Table ​ Table1). 1 ). To that end, a large-scale meta-review that assembled original perspectives, theories and definitions of social change within both the sociological and psychological literature was performed. The typology of social change that emerged distinguishes four separate social contexts associated with social change: stability, inertia, incremental social change, and DSC. DSC, because of its frequency in today's world, and because it is threatening to people, requires special attention. Thus, the proposed typology of social change drills deeper and articulates four necessary characteristics for a change or an event to be labeled as “dramatic social change”: rapid pace of change, rupture in social structure, rupture in normative structure, and threat to cultural identity. Finally, I come full circle by proposing a theoretical model that links together the four characteristics of DSC within the proposed typology of social change (see Figure ​ Figure1). 1 ). In sum, the typology of social change I am suggesting can be useful to create a theoretical consensus among researchers about what social change is that perhaps will allow for a coordinated, evidence based strategy to address the psychology of social change.

The typology of social change .

StabilityA situation where an event, regardless of its pace, does not affect the equilibrium of a society's social and normative structures nor the cultural identity of group members. The event, may, however, impact an isolated number of individuals.
InertiaA situation where an event, regardless of its pace, does not either reinstate the equilibrium of a society's social and normative structures or clarify the cultural identity of group members.
Incremental social changeA situation where a slow event leads to a gradual but profound societal transformation and slowly changes the social and/or the normative structure or changes/threatens the cultural identity of group members.
Dramatic social changeA situation where a rapid event leads to a profound societal transformation and produces a rupture in the equilibrium of the social and normative structures and changes/threatens the cultural identity of group members.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fpsyg-08-00397-g0001.jpg

Proposed theoretical model .

Social change in sociology and psychology

Today, the field of sociology is at the forefront of social change theory and research, with a particular focus upon the factors that constitute and are prerequisites to social change. Within the sociological literature, three main theories have been championed for their attempt to explain social change: Evolutionary Theory, Conflict Theory , and Functionalist Theory . Each theory is characterized by key descriptive interpretations in Table ​ Table2 2 where a global overview of the conceptualization of social change is offered 1 .

Theories of social change in sociology .

Evolutionary theorySociety moves in a linear direction from a simple to a more complex structure.Comte, ; Spencer, ; Pareto,
Conflict theoryIndividuals and their groups fight to maximize their benefits. Society is in a constant state of disequilibrium.Marx and Engels,
Functionalist theorySociety is in a constant state of equilibrium. When a change occurs in one part of society, adjustments are made. Social change occurs when the equilibrium is compromised due to the rapidity with which events occur.Durkheim, ; Parson,

Despite the first appearance of “social change” in the psychological literature more than 70 years ago, only a few isolated psychologists have focused on social change per se and even fewer have offered a clear definition or conceptualization of the concept. The first paper that defined social change was published in the Academy of Political and Social Science and was entitled Psychology of Social Change . Social change was defined as “always a slow and gradual process” (Marquis, 1947 , p. 75). From that point in time to the dissolution of the Soviet Union in 1991, there have been very few attempts to reintroduce social change into the field of psychology (e.g., Pizer and Travers, 1975 ; Schneiderman, 1988 ). However, after the dissolution of the Soviet Union and the fall of the Berlin Wall, there has been a small surge of research on social change in psychology. For example, several edited books (e.g., Thomas and Veno, 1992 ; Breakwell and Lyons, 1996 ; Crockett and Silbereisen, 2000 ) and special issues of journals (Silbereisen and Tomasik, 2010 ; Blackwood et al., 2013 ) have focused exclusively on social change and on people's reactions to it. For clarity purposes, Table ​ Table3 3 attempts to summarize the various theories or perspectives in different subfields of cultural and social psychology while Table ​ Table4 4 attempts to do so in subfields of psychology.

Theories and perspectives addressing social change in social psychology .

Social Identity Theory (SIT)Social identity relies on two aspects that may be associated with social change. First, SIT is a theory of social structure that is based on perceptions of legitimacy, stability, and permeability. Second, SIT proposes identity management strategies such as collective action whereby minority groups aim to maintain or acquire a positive and distinctive social identity.Tajfel and Turner,
Social Dominance Orientation (SDO)In terms of SDO, social change can be interpreted as the opposition of hierarchy-enhancing attitudes in individuals with high SDO and hierarchy-attenuating ones in individuals with low SDO.Sidanius and Pratto,
Relative Deprivation Theory (RDT)RDT can be applied to social change in two distinct ways. First, collective relative deprivation occurs when people compare their group to other groups and feel that their group is worse off which will motivate them to improve their status by means of collective action. Second, in times of DSC, people are usually confronted with a unique situation that results in confusion and the loss of social cues. It is therefore easier and more relevant for them to compare their group's present situation to their group's status at another well-defined time period, than to compare their group with another group. Recent research proposes the use of a historical trajectory when assessing one's group's collective relative deprivation.Runciman, ; de la Sablonnière et al., ,
Immigration and Identity Integration (III)Immigration is a form of social change that requires human adaptation. Research in this field has demonstrated that individuals who simultaneously identify with their culture of origin and with the receiving group's culture and also desire contact with both cultures experience the highest levels of well-being.Benet-Martínez and Haritatos, ; Berry, ; Amiot et al.,
Identity Process Theory (IPT)IPT explores the structure of an individual's identity and the coping strategies used when facing an identity threat or change that results from social change.Breakwell,
System Justification Theory (SJT)SJT is a theory that explains how to preserve the status quo. It's more a theory of stability than of social change. Both advantaged and disadvantaged individuals endorse system-justifying ideologies, to preserve the existing social structure.Jost et al.,
Identity Threat Theory (ITT)In ITT, when a threat to identity occurs as a result of social change, individuals will regulate the structure of their identity by restoring the imbalance and modifying their identity through different processes that include integrating the new elements into their identity and assigning a positive or negative valence to them.Steele et al.,
Adjustment to Change Theory (ACT)ACT considers how individuals adjust to social change and argues that factors such as social support and the nature of the event predict the way individuals and groups evaluate social change.Goodwin,

Theories addressing social change in subfields of psychology .

Cultural and evolutionary psychologyFocuses on how social change and human biology are linked and aims to identify how social change influences human genetics and the way humans adapt to these changes.Feldman and Laland, ; Laland et al.,
Developmental psychologyResearch in this field has demonstrated that social change has the potential to impact developmental stages for children and adolescents as well as their identities and well-being.Pinquart and Silbereisen, ; Greenfield, ,
Industrial/organizational psychologyFocuses on organizational change as a form of social change. Three main themes emerge from this field: how to successfully implement organizational change, how to limit the negative impact of organizational change and understand the psychological processes of people who are confronting organizational change.Kanter, ; Burke and Litwin, ; Sanzgiri and Gottlieb, ; Meyer and Allen, ; Reichers et al.,

Limitations of current research and conceptualization of social change in sociology and psychology

As indicated in the summary tables, both contemporary and traditional theorists in sociology and psychology have addressed social change through a variety of macro sociological or societal lenses, and equally from a plethora of micro, psychological, or individual perspectives. Theory and research thus far has demonstrated that social change is a complex entity (e.g., McGrath, 1983 ; Buchanan et al., 2005 ; Subašić et al., 2012 ) that can be conceptualized in many diverging (and confusing) ways. The challenge associated with defining social change may well be to explain why it is an understudied phenomenon (de la Sablonnière et al., 2013 ) and highlight the challenge of moving forward in studying its psychological impact on ordinary people. The typology of social change presented here offers an initial attempt at clarifying the meaning of social change from a psychological perspective. That is, I focus on an individualistic perspective, but attempt to address the role that macro processes play in terms of our more micro or psychological focus. Here, I discuss three main issues that point to the necessity to properly conceptualize DSC.

First, and most importantly, the conceptualization and understanding of social change does not reach a consensus within the scientific literature (e.g., Coughlin and Khinduka, 1976 ). Furthermore, few scientists define precisely what they mean when using the concept (e.g., Saran, 1963 ). For example, when social change is studied from a social identity theory perspective (Tajfel and Turner, 1986 ), or a sociological conflict theory perspective, social change is conceptualized almost exclusively in the context of collective action (Krznaric, 2007 ). In light of this, collective action is defined as a means for group members to achieve an improved social position for their group in the social hierarchy (Taylor and McKirnan, 1984 ; Batel and Castro, 2015 ; de Lemus and Stroebe, 2015 ). In contrast, cultural psychology and developmental psychology conceptualize social change in a broader manner (e.g., societal transformations such as the fall of the Soviet Union; immigration) where change is not limited to the context of intergroup conflict (Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004 ; Sun and Ryder, 2016 ). The fact that there is divergence in conceptualizing social change is preventing coordinated research on social change, because not all types of social change are considered. With some theories (e.g., relative deprivation theory, social identity theory, evolutionary theory, conflict theory), social change is conceived mostly as an autonomously controlled and unidirectional process toward group change; these conceptualizations do not account for social changes that are outside of human control, such as natural disasters (e.g., Coughlin and Khinduka, 1976 ). Equating social change with collective action (see Stroebe et al., 2015 ), for example, neglects uncontrollable social transformations such as socio-political reforms and natural disasters over which individuals or groups exert no control. Indeed, the majority of individuals who experience DSC have little control over such events. Since previous classifications can only explain some instances of social change, a theory that would clarify the characteristics required in conceptualizing DSC for all types of change has become a necessity.

The second issue that points to the need for a typology of social change is that not all social contexts associated with social change (i.e., stability and inertia) were considered in previous scientific literature. Most theoretical and empirical work on social change in both sociology and psychology has focused on either incremental social change or DSC (e.g., Andersson et al., 2014 ; Bernstrøm and Kjekshus, 2015 ). However, in order to have a complete theory or typology of social change, it is also necessary to take into account social contexts where there is no social change, contexts of either stability or inertia (Table ​ (Table1). 1 ). Knowing about incremental social change, inertia and stability, as well as how they relate to DSC is psychologically critical. A clear definition of the four social contexts of social change can facilitate finding solutions for the population to not only the consequences associated with DSC, but also the considerable and potentially unique challenges associated with each of these social contexts (see Abrams and Vasiljevic, 2014 ). For example, a society in a state of inertia may be misconceived as a society in a state of DSC if no clear understanding of each social context is achieved. In inertia, there might be less hope for reverting to a healthy society and consequently less long-term goals that are developed, whereas a time of DSC, such as a political revolution, may provide some hope for the future and some possibilities for some concrete long-term goals. Although the main focus of our paper is DSC, the full spectrum of social contexts associated with social change is presented. A more comprehensive theory of social change capable of accounting for stability, inertia as well as incremental and DSC is required to fully understand the psychological processes and ramifications of social change. Moreover, it is important to define stability, inertia, and incremental social change because they serve as a base for comparison or contrast to DSC. As Calhoun notes: “To understand social change, thus, it is necessary also to understand what produces social continuity” (Calhoun, 2000 , p. 2642).

Finally, the third issue that pushes me to develop a typology of social change is that, mainly in sociology, a specific event that can be characterized as social change can be interpreted in light of different theories of social change. Let us take the 2005 Tulip Revolution in Kyrgyzstan as an example. Evolutionary theorists may argue that this revolution followed the natural evolution of Kyrgyz society. On the other hand, functionalist theorists may argue that there was disequilibrium in Kyrgyzstan at the time of the revolution. However, it would be beneficial to conceptualize social change the same way in order to be able to assess its impact on individuals. What is needed is a conceptualization of social change that can be interpreted in light of all the theories and processes that have been developed thus far. When an in-depth analysis of the literature is performed, the essential characteristics that define social change across theories may be ascertained. For example, one of the characteristics that was identified in conceptualizing DSC was the rapid pace of social change. The rapid vs. slow pace of social change is important, for instance, to distinguish a DSC from an incremental social change where transformations in the social structure take place without major disruptions. Whether one conceptualizes social change from a functionalist theory, a social identity theory, or a developmental theory perspective, most researchers from these distinctive fields point to the pace of change as one pivotal and essential element that characterizes DSC. Thus, when I base the typology of social change upon such characteristics, garnered from previous research in both sociology and psychology, an all-encompassing conceptualization of social change may be obtained, and later used to guide empirical research independently of the diverging theoretical perspectives.

My observations on the limitations of sociology and psychology should not detract from the insightful contributions these disciplines have made to our understanding of social change. Indeed, these social scientists have tapped into very important issues. For example, although collective action is not the only type of social change, the research on this topic has successfully identified factors that lead individuals and groups to be dissatisfied with their conditions and engage in collective action. However, as Sampson ( 1989 ) pointed out: “we have not gone far enough in connecting our theories of the person with social change, in particular, with major historic transformation in the social world” (p. 417). Since our contemporary social world is characterized by social change (Weinstein, 2010 ), like Sampson ( 1989 ), I argue that “a psychology for tomorrow is a psychology that begins actively to chart out a theory of the person that is no longer rooted in the liberal individualistic assumptions, but is reframed in terms more suitable to resolving the issues of a global era” (p. 431).

In sum, social change needs to be clearly examined because future research is limited without an all-inclusive typology of social change; one that can bridge the epistemological differences between theories from various fields of research and diverging theoretical perspectives. What is needed is a clear conceptualization of social change that considers, and includes, the different characteristics that compose DSC and that were suggested by researchers from all these diverging areas and theoretical orientations.

Constructing a typology of social change: the characteristics of DSC

Two separate databases from sociology and psychology were targeted to collate relevant peer-reviewed publications: Sociology Abstracts and PsycInfo. Including the year 2016, a total of 5,676 abstracts were carefully analyzed (90% inter-judge reliability; Table ​ Table5). 5 ). Two inclusion criteria were used to determine if a manuscript was relevant to our typology of social change. First, the selected abstract, and then the articles, needed to a) focus on social change by including a relevant original definition or providing an original perspective on the concept (originality), or b) focus on one's perspective of social change at either the individual or group level (perceptions).

Number of abstracts and articles that satisfied the specified inclusion criteria .

Evaluated abstracts28142862
Accepted abstracts250178
Missing articles1216
Read and accepted articles161114
Other articles and books50
Total325

When reviewing the literature, I had one main goal: selecting and identifying the necessary characteristics of DSC that could either be present or not in other social contexts (i.e., stability, inertia, and incremental social change). Scientists refer to the characteristics in two different ways: (1) formally, when defining or describing DSC, incremental social change, stability, or inertia, and (2) informally, when introducing their research on social change 2 . I made sure that the included articles sufficiently addressed one or more of the four selected characteristics (i.e., rapid pace of change, rupture in social structure, rupture in normative structure, and threat to cultural identity, see Table ​ Table6). 6 ). These four characteristics were chosen after a first reading of each of the articles (up to October 2013). They emerged most consistently and were singled out more often for their importance. From prior knowledge, I anticipated that “pace of change” and “social structure” would surface. The other two emerged naturally. From prior knowledge, I also expected the term “valence of change” (i.e., negative change) to emerge (e.g., Slone et al., 2002 ; de la Sablonnière and Tougas, 2008 ; de la Sablonnière et al., 2009c ; Kim, 2008 ). However, that characteristic did not appear in a significant number of papers. The fact that some authors report “positive” change as having negative consequences (e.g., Prislin and Christensen, 2005 ; Bruscella, 2015 ) and “negative” change as having positive consequence (e.g., Yakushko, 2008 ; Abrams and Vasiljevic, 2014 ) may explain why the valence did not emerge as an important characteristic of DSC.

Characteristics of dramatic social change .

1. The pace of changeThe speed at which an event impacts a collectivity.185
2. Rupture in the social structureA break with the past so that even core aspects of society such as social institutions have to be reconstructed; a society undergoes a complete transformation.196
3. Rupture in the normative structureA break with the past in terms of the core behaviors of the group members that now have to be modified significantly in order to achieve collective goals.195
4. Cultural identity threatA serious threat to identification and to the clarity of the shared beliefs, values, attitudes, and behavioral scripts associated with one's group.205

To conceptualize an event as DSC, all four characteristics must be present. For example, if an event is affecting only the normative structure in a gradual manner, it would not be possible to label that event as DSC. As for the other three social contexts (stability, inertia, and incremental social change), each has its own unique configuration of characteristics (see Figure ​ Figure1 1 ) 3 .

The pace of change

The first characteristic that emerged regards the pace , which could either be slow or rapid, and is defined as the speed at which an event impacts a collectivity . When defining social change, researchers from both sociology and psychology distinguish two types of social change based on the pace of change: incremental (e.g., first-order change, beta change, decline, gradual, small-scale) and dramatic (e.g., second-order, gamma, abrupt, collapse, large-scale).

Theories of social change have explicitly and/or implicitly acknowledged the pace of social change as a central determining factor toward its characterization. For example, in one of the earliest versions of their seminal book, Lenski and Lenski ( 1974 ) state: “The most striking feature of contemporary life is the revolutionary pace of social change. Never before have things changed so fast for so much of mankind” (Lenski and Lenski, 1974 , p. 3, see also Fried, 1964 ; Rudel and Hooper, 2005 ). In their new edition entitled Human Societies: An Introduction to Macrosociology , Nolan and Lenski ( 2011 ) describe how slowly human evolution has progressed for thousands of years until about 100 years ago, when humans began to evolve at an accelerated pace. Similarly, Weinstein ( 2010 ) suggests that for the last few decades, there has been “rapid and accelerating rates of change in human relations, from the interpersonal to the international level” (p. xvii).

It is worthwhile to note that a few key authors refer to pace when distinguishing different types of social change. For example, in organizational psychology, Nadler and Tushman ( 1995 ) distinguish slow “incremental” change from fast “discontinuous” change, where the latter would be characterized as DSC in the typology of social change. According to these authors, incremental changes are intended to continually improve the fit among the components of an organization. These changes can either be small or large; nonetheless, there is a succession of manageable changes and adaptation processes. In contrast, discontinuous changes are often linked to major changes in the global scope of the industry and involve a complete break with the past as well as a major reconstruction of almost all elements of the organization. These changes are more traumatic, painful, and demanding as individuals are required to acquire a whole new set of behaviors and discard old patterns. These dramatic changes are not made to improve the fit, but to construct a new collectivity, be it a nation-state, institution or sub-group of the larger collectivity. Newman ( 2000 ) also distinguishes between first-order change and second-order change in the context of organizations. According to him, a first-order change, which is equivalent to incremental social change, “is most likely during times of relative environmental stability and is likely to take place over extended periods of time” (Newman, 2000 , p.604). In other words, this type of change occurs slowly and allows the organization and its members to adapt to the changes gradually. However, a second-order change, or DSC, is radical, and transforms the core of the organization (Newman, 2000 ). In this case, the change is so sudden that it does not necessarily allow individuals to adapt to the process (Buchanan et al., 2005 ). Similarly, Rogers ( 2003 ) defines social change as abrupt and arises when the entire system is modified and jeopardized because changes are too fast for the system to adjust. In his book, Diamond ( 2005 ) contrasts “decline”—where minor ups and downs do not restructure the society—with “collapse”—an extreme form of several milder types of decline—which make it a DSC. An example of collapse is when most of the inhabitants of a population vanish as a result of ecological disasters, starvation, war, or disease. Examples of this are genocides such as Rwanda's which claimed around 800,000 lives, destroyed much of the country's infrastructure and displaced four million people (Des Forges, 1999 ; Zorbas, 2004 ; Pham et al., 2004 ; Staub et al., 2005 ; Schaal and Elbert, 2006 ; Prunier, 2010 ; Yanagizawa-Drott, 2014 ), the Armenian Massacres, which saw the systematic extermination of about 1.5 million minority Armenians in Turkey (Dadrian, 1989 , 1998 ) or Cambodia's genocide, which involved the death of almost two million people through the Khmer Rouge's policies of relocation, mass executions, torture, forced labor, malnutrition, and disease (Hannum, 1989 ). All these events led to an inordinate number of deaths and population movements in a short, restricted period of time.

To be considered dramatic , a social change needs to be quick and must involve a “break with the past” (Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ; see also Armenakis et al., 1986 ). The example most often used in the literature is the breakdown of the communist system in Eastern Europe and the Soviet Union (e.g., Kollontai, 1999 ; Pinquart et al., 2009 ; Round and Williams, 2010 ; Walker and Stephenson, 2010 ; Chen, 2015 ). For example, when Pinquart et al. ( 2004 , p. 341) introduced their research on social change, they made a distinction between “gradual” change, such as ideological change in many Western societies, and “abrupt social change,” which represents a form of social change that may be spurred by a sudden, dramatic transformation of economic, political, and social institutions.

Rupture in the social structure

The second characteristic of DSC that emerges from my review regards a rupture in the social structure of a collectivity or a group. Social structure is a term that has several different uses in the sociological literature and this is, in part, because of the lack of agreement on how the term social structure should be defined (Porpora, 1989 ; López and Scott, 2000 ). One main dispute pits the dualism of “action” (or agency) vs. “structure” in mainstream sociological work (for a discussion see López and Scott, 2000 ). Consequently, many of the definitions describe behaviors rather than the role of social institutions (e.g., Cortina et al., 2012 ; Tanner and Jackson, 2012 ; Wilson, 2012 ). For example, Tanner and Jackson ( 2012 ) define social structure as “the formation of groups via connections among individuals” (p. 260), which focuses on meso-level interactions among individuals. Similarly, Macionis et al. ( 2008 ) define social structure as “any relatively stable pattern of social behavior” (p. 13).

The social structure being discussed in the present paper refers to macro-level elements of society such as institutions that facilitate and structure collective interactions, roles or behaviors. Thus, directly inspired from the most prominent definitions of social structure in the literature (Marx, 1859/1970 ; Giddens, 1979 ; Porpora, 1989 ; López and Scott, 2000 ; Stinchcombe, 2000 ), social structure is defined here as a system of socio-economic stratification, social institutions, organizations, national policies and laws that help structure the norms, roles, behaviors, and values of community members 4 .

In both sociology and psychology, a rupture in the social structure is at the heart of definitions of social change. For example, for Breakwell and Lyons ( 1996 ), changes involve the disintegration of previous national and international order and sets in motion a process of re-definition and re-evaluation of societal norms, belief systems, and power structures. While the communal sense of continuity and permanence is challenged, social change often represents a period of massive transformations in political, social, and economic structures (e.g., Goodwin, 1998 ; Kim and Ng, 2008 ; Chen, 2012 ). This conceptualization is similar to the definition inspired by sociologists and provided by Silbereisen and Tomasik ( 2010 , p. 243) where “social change is understood as a more or less rapid and comprehensive change of societal structures and institutions, including changes to the economic, technological, and cultural frameworks of a society (Calhoun, 1992 )” or to Kohn's definition of radical social change: “we refer not to the pace of change but to the nature of the change—the transformation of one political and economic system into a quite different system” (Kohn et al., 1997 , p. 615).

When research focuses on collective action, social structure is placed at the root of their definition. For example, “Breakdown Theories” in sociology argue that social movements result from the disruption or breakdown of previously integrative social structures. This theory regards collective action as a form of social imbalance that results from the improper functioning of social institutions (Tilly et al., 1975 ). Macionis et al. ( 2008 ) also suggest that, “revolutionary social movements attempt to target the whole collectivity by radically changing social institutions” (p. 452). Put differently, for social movements and collective action to occur, social institutions—consequently, the social structure of society—needs to be altered. In other words, social change “is the sudden shifting of power from group to group” (Schrickel, 1945 , p. 188). To many authors, DSC involves a rupture in the social structure (e.g., Prilleltensky, 1990 ) where people need to “negotiate their way through or around social structures” (May, 2011 , p. 367).

Rupture in the normative structure

The third characteristic of DSC that emerged from the literature is the rupture in the normative structure of society. While reading on the subject, I noticed an important distinction between social structure and normative structure. As mentioned in the previous section, that distinction pointed to a duality that is also observed by theorists in sociology who attempt to define social structure (e.g., Giddens, 1979 ; Mayhew, 1980 ; Porpora, 1989 ; López and Scott, 2000 ). Although both the social and normative structures refer to the functioning of a society, they each point to two different aspects of communities and groups. As discussed earlier, the social structure is associated with macro processes such as social institutions (e.g., Government), whereas the normative structure is related to micro processes as they principally refer to community members' habitual behaviors and norms.

Based on the work of Taylor and de la Sablonnière ( 2013 , 2014 ), the normative structure is defined here as the behaviors of most community members whose aim is achieving collective goals . In other terms, when the normative structure is clear, people know what to do and when to engage in specific behaviors in order to meet the overarching goals of the collectivity. The definition of normative structure also takes its inspiration from an array of different domains in the scientific literature. Mainly, it comes from the definitions of social change that most often involve a change in behaviors and habits that are disrupted with the event of a dramatic and rapid social change. For example, Bishop ( 1998 , p. 406) clearly states that social change in its transformational form refers to “the ability of a group to behave differently, even to creating brand-new elements, within the same social identity.” This definition concurs with definitions of many more authors, such as Delanty's ( 2012 ) concept of “normative culture” or May's (2011), where the mundane “ordinary” activities take a central place in social change.

Research and theories on social change have put normative structure as one of its central tenants. For example, Tomasik et al. ( 2010 ), argue that social change involves “changes of the macro-context that disturb habits, interrupt routines, or require novel behaviors relevant for a successful mastery” (p. 247). These authors also assert that when a gradual social change occurs, “old options of thinking and behaving are usually still available whereas abrupt social change is often associated with an immediate blocking of old options” (Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004 , p. 295). Therefore, in the latter case, it will be necessary to develop new ways of doing things.

Jerneić and Šverko ( 2001 ) argue that “major political and socioeconomic changes may strongly influence people's life role priorities, which are otherwise relatively stable behavioral dispositions” (p. 46). In fact, the normative structure of a society is comprised not only of norms and behaviors, but also of roles that people have in their everyday lives. When a DSC occurs, these normative elements of people's lives are all greatly affected to the point where they need to be redefined. Similarly, McDade and Worthman ( 2004 ) refer to “socialization ambiguity,” a state present in the context of DSC where “inconsistent messages or conflicting expectations regarding appropriate beliefs and social behavior during the course of socialization may be a substantial source of stress for the developing individual” (p. 52; see also Arnett, 1995 ; Tonkens, 2012 ).

This rupture in the normative structure of society is present not only when radical changes such as natural disasters occur, but also when social change is the result of collective actions within a society. Subašić et al. ( 2012 ) acknowledge that “what we do is evidently shaped by social norms, by institutional possibilities, and institutional constraints. But equally, we can act—act together that is—to alter norms, institutions, and even whole social systems” (p. 66). Therefore, when members of a society come together and engage in collective actions, an important aspect of society they aim to change deals with the norms and normative structure.

The importance of the normative component involved in DSC is in accordance with the Normative Theory of Social Change, developed by Taylor et al. (Taylor and de la Sablonnière, 2013 , 2014 ; see also de la Sablonnière et al., 2009b ). According to their theory, any group—whether it be at the collective, community or country level—functions along the basic 80-20 principle in times of stability. According to this principle, most of the citizens in a functioning society (i.e., 80% of them) will exhibit normative behaviors that agree with the normative structure of the society in order to accomplish collective goals such as achieving a healthy society, and by extension, personal goals such as maintaining a healthy lifestyle. It is the 80% that provide social support, when necessary, to the 20% of citizens who do not function successfully in the society. In theory, as long as there is a decent majority of people who conform to the normative structure, a society should function relatively smoothly. Unfortunately, this is not always the case. Sometimes, when a society is confronted with DSC, its normative structure is ruptured which may lead to societal dysfunction or important disruptions in the “usual” behavior of group members. In such a situation, the amount of group members exhibiting behaviors that are in agreement with the collective goals of the group will be lower than usual. Therefore, it is possible that instead of having 80% of group members acting according to the normative rules of the society, only 30 or 40 % of individuals will follow these rules. In this case, it becomes very difficult for people to restore the functional equilibrium of the normative structure as only a few group members are in a position to provide the necessary social support for the entire society to function properly (Taylor and de la Sablonnière, 2014 ). What is suggested here is consistent with the work of Albert and Sabini ( 1974 ). These authors refer to the importance of a supportive environment, or social support, which has a sufficient presence in “slow change,” but not when the context is one of rapid change.

Threat to cultural identity

The fourth characteristic of social change is threats to the cultural identity of a group. This characteristic is a difficult one to label since different authors use different terms to describe a threat to cultural identity (i.e., lack of clarity, identity conflict, identity crisis, lowered identification, identity confusion). As opposed to terms such as identity conflict, identity crisis, lack of identity clarity and identity change, “threat to cultural identity” was chosen for its capacity to suggest a potential modification in identity. To be considered DSC, the cultural identity in its current form must somehow be jeopardized, challenged, or lowered. Values and beliefs are, per se , questioned and the individual may sense a general lack of clarity and feel threatened to the core of his group identity, value system, or beliefs.

Many scientists have defined and researched collective and/or cultural identity. Recently, Ashmore et al. ( 2004 ) have defined collective identity as “first and foremost a statement about categorical membership. A collective identity is one that is shared with a group of others who have (or are believed to have) some characteristic(s) in common” (p. 81). This definition is similar to the one from Taylor ( 1997 ), in which cultural identity is referred to as the beliefs about shared rules and behaviors (Taylor, 1997 , 2002 ; Usborne and de la Sablonnière, 2014 ).

When a social change occurs, it threatens the cultural identity of all community members. In the present paper, inspired from previous work on cultural identity, I define threat to cultural identity as a serious threat to identification and to the clarity of the shared beliefs, values, attitudes, and behavioral scripts associated with one's group . Throughout the literature I reviewed, cultural identity threat was manifested according to three main themes. The first theme that stood out is that threats to identity are associated with a loss of identity or an identity change (e.g., subtractive identification pattern; de la Sablonnière et al., 2016 ). Some authors directly mention the threat to cultural identity within the context of major social change (e.g., Vaughan, 1986 ; Smelser and Swedberg, 1994 ; Sztompka, 2000 ; Wyn and White, 2000 ; Van Binh, 2002 ; Terry and Jimmieson, 2003 ). For example, in his paper on how cultures change as a function of mass immigration Moghaddam ( 2012 ) argues that globalization results in sudden contact among different groups of people from different countries. This form of sudden contact has often resulted in the extinction of many cultures and languages such as Indigenous peoples around the world. Therefore, globalization makes people feel that their collective identity is threatened. Specifically, they experience a loss in many components of their cultural identity including their values and their language (see also Van Binh, 2002 ). The process described by Moghaddam is similar to the one proposed by Lapuz ( 1976 ) who argues that when social change occurs rapidly, people's beliefs and values are threatened since the old guidelines are no longer available. One consequence of this threat is that people become confused as values and beliefs contribute to the emotional security and psychological survival of individuals (Lapuz, 1976 ; Varnum, 2008 ). This is in agreement with Albert's ( 1977 ) proposition: “Rapid change constitutes a major threat to self-identity” (p. 499). Similarly, in their book entitled Changing European Identities , Breakwell and Lyons ( 1996 ) discuss the mechanisms associated with change in identities in the context of the development of the European Union and refer to a loss of national identity. This change in cultural identity is similar to what Wall and Louchakova ( 2002 ) describe as a “shift in the cultural collective consciousness” (p.253). This consists of a change in the American self and the emergence of new selves, more independent and alive in the context of change (see also Neves and Caetano, 2009 ; May, 2011 ).

The second theme is associated with the lack of identity clarity in the event of DSC. This lack of clarity is due to uncertainties or inconsistencies in the definition of one's identity. A clear cultural identity is defined as “the extent to which beliefs about one's group are clearly and confidently defined” (Usborne and Taylor, 2010 , p. 883; see also Taylor, 2002 ). It has been theorized and demonstrated that an unclear cultural identity can result in lower self-esteem (Usborne and Taylor, 2010 ). Thus, if the entire collective is experiencing an unclear cultural identity, it may affect people's ability to function effectively in their society. Similarly, Macionis et al. ( 2008 ) refer to inconsistencies in the context of socialization in times of important change. People try to seek out new roles, try new “selves” (Macionis et al., 2008 , p.461). They need to adapt to the inconsistent model their societies are projecting, which leads to “socialization ambiguity” (McDade and Worthman, 2004 , p. 49). Because social change brings uncertainty in society, it can affect many aspects of individuals' lives such as family relations (Noak et al., 2001 ), and aspects associated with the self such as “emotions, values, perceptions, identity” (Wall and Louchakova, 2002 , p. 266).

Finally, as a third theme, authors refer to conflicting identities within the context of dramatic contextual change. For example, Becker conducted a study to find out how rapid social change, such as introducing television in a community that had never owned televisions before, would impact body images of girls and women in that community (Becker, 2004 ). She found that television caused confusion and conflicts about ideal body images, and consequently “reshap[ed] [their] personal and cultural identities” (Becker, 2004 , p. 551). In some cases, it even led to eating disorders (Becker, 2004 ), which has a direct link with the way people evaluate and perceive themselves. In other words, this DSC altered their identity. In fact, severe contextual changes can challenge the meaning of identity and threaten its existence (Ethier and Deaux, 1994 ; Macek et al., 2013 ). Similarly, Hoffman and Medlock-Klyukovski ( 2004 ) argue that contemporary organizations are “typically marked by conflicting interests and contradictory demands on individuals” (p. 389). This is similar to Chen ( 2012 ) who refers to the need for a transformation and the need to create new cultural norms and values when confronted to the context of social change (Chen, 2012 ).

The typology of social change

In order to properly conceptualize DSC and other social contexts associated with the state of a collectivity, I suggest a typology of social change comprised of four different social contexts: “stability,” “inertia,” “incremental social change,” and “DSC” (see Table ​ Table1 1 for definitions). These social contexts are consistent with the theoretical stance of a large number of sociologists (e.g., Durkheim, 1893/1967 , 1897/1967 ; Watzlawick et al., 1974 ; Rocher, 1992 ; Fukuyama, 1999 ; Rogers, 2003 ; May, 2011 ; Nolan and Lenski, 2011 ), psychologists (e.g., Katz, 1974 ; Moghaddam, 2002 ; Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004 ; Goodwin, 2006 ; de la Sablonnière et al., 2009a ) and scientists in the field of organizational behavior (e.g., Golembiewski et al., 1976 ; Tushman and Romanelli, 1985 ; Armenakis et al., 1986 ; Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ; Thompson and Hunt, 1996 ).

As many different concepts surround each of the four social contexts, it was necessary to choose a meaningful label for each. For “stability” and “inertia,” the choice was relatively easy because these two labels are commonly used and applied consistently. The term “status quo” was also considered rather than “stability” (e.g., Prilleltensky, 1990 ; Diekman and Goodfriend, 2007 ; Mucchi-Faina et al., 2010 ). However, because there could also be “status quo” in the context of inertia (e.g., Subašić et al., 2008 ), the term “stability” was preferred.

When it came to “incremental” and “dramatic” social change, the decision was more arduous as authors from different research fields use different labels. For example, instead of referring to “DSC,” Golembiewski et al. ( 1976 ) refers to “gamma changes”; Nadler and Tushman ( 1995 ), to “discontinuous change.” Others refer to “second-order change” (Watzlawick et al., 1974 ; Bartunek and Moch, 1987 ; Bate, 1994 ; Newman, 2000 ), to “abrupt” (e.g., Back, 1971 ; Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004 ) or even to “rapid” change (e.g., Becker, 2004 ; McDade and Worthman, 2004 ). The term “dramatic” social change was chosen for its ability to clearly and distinctively define the situation confronting ordinary people. In a similar fashion, the term “incremental” social change was preferred over the labels: “first-order change,” “beta change,” and “continuous change.”

When there is stability , the actual state of a society is maintained and the majority of group members are actively attempting to attain society's goals. As Weinstein ( 2010 ) describes it, it is a state in which “the established order appears to be operating effectively, and disturbing influences from within or from other societies are insignificant” (p. 9; see also Bess ( 2015 ) where no change is equated with stability). Indeed, none of the four characteristic of social change are present. For example, the social and normative structures fluctuate little, and changes do not affect what is defined as normal behavior in a community (Harmon et al., 2015 ). Indeed, personal change, such as bereavement or divorce, still occurs for some members of society. However, in the event of a personal change, the social or normative structures are not disrupted, mainly because the collective social support system remains functional and people can rely on that support in case they experience changes in their individual lives. This is also consistent with the findings of Albert and Sabini ( 1974 ) who argue that changes occurring in a supportive environment or in a peripheral element of society are perceived as less disruptive than those occurring in a non-supportive environment because the strain upon society is attenuated.

Consistent with previous research, stability can be defined as a situation where an event, regardless of its pace, does not affect the equilibrium of a society's social and normative structures nor the cultural identity of group members. The event, may, however, impact an isolated number of individuals . An example that might clarify this definition of stability is the event of an election. Although many people can get excited and seem to be affected by this event, an election does not necessarily bring about a rupture in a society, even if it involves a change of political party. The core elements of society remain stable and citizens resume their activities without feeling their lives have been overly disrupted by the election and its outcome. If, for instance, supporters of the defeated party feel sad and hopeless about the defeat, plenty of other citizens will be available to help them cope since most of them will not be affected by the change of government. However, in a different context, the event of an election may trigger DSC; for example, when it leads to a social revolution.

In contrast with stability, a context where there is inertia involves a situation that does affect a large number of people, if not most of the people composing a society. Inertia is defined as a situation where an event, regardless of its pace, does not either reinstate the equilibrium of a society's social and normative structures or clarify the cultural identity of group members .

In times of inertia, if a “positive” event occurs, there is no sustainability to maintain its positive impact. Here, the example of Belarus is used, a country where the population has been in a state of inertia since the fall of the Soviet Union. Lukashenko has been the president of the country since 1994. Under his autocratic rule, Belarus is known as the last dictatorship in Europe. Many Belarusians are longing for a more democratic and open society, yet the country remains in inertia. Buchanan et al. ( 2005 ) describe a situation of inertia as an “absence of appropriate activity, a lack of capability, a failure to pay attention to signals, and thus as an impediment rather than a desired condition” (p. 190). Inertia is seen as an undesirable situation where constructive change is not possible because the organization (or the group) does not have the capacity (e.g., lack of resources or will) to carry out the needed change. These authors also argue that when a change is implemented, its sustainability requires managers and staff (or community members) to share the same objectives. Uncertainty about the future must be minimal.

Accordingly, one can assume that the criteria underpinning sustainability in the event of a change are already absent in a society that has stagnated due to inertia. Therefore, inertia in a society such as Belarus constitutes a context where the population is uncertain about the future and does not share the same long-term goals as its government. There is a desire for positive social change, but the actual structure of the society makes it difficult for any change to be implemented and be sustained. Indeed, for a positive change to be maintained, it must have the support of individuals in power since they have the appropriate resources to address society's problems. Unsurprisingly, sustainability of such a change is threatened by an autocratic style of governing (Buchanan et al., 2005 ).

In sum, inertia differs from stability. In the case of inertia, most members of society desire a change from the actual state of their group, but are unable to properly sustain change due to a lack of collective social support and an unclear cultural identity. In contrast, in the case of “stability,” the society functions in an efficient manner when meeting the collective goals.

Incremental social change

Incremental social change is defined as a situation where a slow event leads to a gradual but profound societal transformation and slowly changes the social and/or the normative structure or changes/threatens the cultural identity of group members . The slow pace is necessary for incremental social change to occur. Moreover, at least one of the other three characteristics needs to occur. In their recent paper, Abrams and Vasiljevic ( 2014 ) speak of “growth,” which could represent one form of incremental social change that involves “wider acceptance of shared values and tolerance of different values” and of “recession” where “disidentification” with current groups can occur (p. 328).

One of the most cited examples of incremental social change is technological innovation (e.g., Rieger, 2003 ; Weinstein, 2010 ; May, 2011 ; Hansen et al., 2012 ). Often, there is no social structural rupture associated with the wide use of technology and normative structure as well as social support remain intact. Given its incremental nature, this type of social change does not instantly produce conflict between old and new behaviors. For instance, when television was introduced, people bought it without knowing the consequences of the implementation of this new technology in their life (Becker, 2004 ; Macionis et al., 2008 ; Weinstein, 2010 ). Today, in retrospect, we know that buying a television set entailed a plethora of new behaviors that altered our society and our way of living. Indeed, some changes in society seem to be a “by-product of our pursuit of other goals and interests” (Subašić et al., 2012 , p. 62). The long time span that is typical for incremental social change makes its outcomes unpredictable and unintentional. For instance, as Weinstein states (Weinstein, 2010 ), “It would be impossible to assess exactly what role electronic telecommunication has played in our global revolution, in part because its effects continue to reverberate and magnify as you read this” (p. 4).

The cell phone is a particularly good example of incremental social change. When it came onto the buyer's market, only a few exclusive people possessed one. However, over the years, it became increasingly normative to have a cell phone and, today, it is almost inconceivable not to have one. Furthermore, when cell phones were first marketed, they were used mainly for business rather than for social purposes, which is the current primary use (Aoki and Downes, 2003 ). In the same vein, other technological changes, such as the emergence of personal computers (Kiesler et al., 1984 ; Robinson et al., 1997 ), Internet (DiMaggio et al., 2001 ; Brignall III and Van Valey, 2005 ), and social media (Robinson et al., 1997 ; O'Keeffe and Clarke-Pearson, 2011 ; Oh et al., 2015 ) will, in the future, be recognized as key events in the historical transformation of social structures and social norms. Such technology does not represent a DSC, but a social change nonetheless as it has modified the way people interact with one another in an incremental manner. As the change occurs for a relatively long period of time, there is consistency in the pattern of change, which allows social structures to adapt and, thus, to remain intact (Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ). Individuals experiencing incremental social change are therefore able to adapt, given that the collective social support is not altered. For example, there is support for people that have yet to possess a cell phone; if they want to buy one, but do not understand how it functions, there are plenty of people that can help them adapt to this new technology. Even if technological change is conceptualized here as an incremental change, it is possible that technology is used to provoke a DSC, for example by instigating an important social revolution (Rodriguez, 2013 ).

Despite technology being the most adequate example, other incremental changes can be observed in other aspects of society such as in medicine. Indeed, advancement in medicine such as effective birth control (Goldin and Katz, 2002 ) was also the cause of a profound incremental social change. The example of contraception is crucial as the pill deeply affected gender roles in society by empowering women by giving them the capacity to control their sexuality. The pill had not only direct positive effects on women's career investments, but also on the opportunity of attending school longer. The pill forever changed women's involvement in our societies and the repercussions of this incremental social change still echo to us through struggles for gender equality, but also in the form of women actively involved in every level of the modern workplace, including higher managements and governmental position. In other words, the gradual nature of incremental social change makes it a profound change in society that neither disturbs the social structure nor the collective social support system.

Dramatic social change

DSC has been defined as “profound societal transformations that produce a complete rupture in the equilibrium of social structures because their adaptive capacities are surpassed” (de la Sablonnière et al., 2009a , p. 325). Although this definition is based on previous sociological work (Parsons, 1964 ; Rocher, 1992 ), it is adapted here according to the four characteristic of DSC. Specifically, I suggest that DSC be defined as a situation where a rapid event leads to a profound societal transformation and produces a rupture in the equilibrium of the social and normative structures and changes/threatens the cultural identity of group members .

As with incremental change, DSC induces fundamental transformations in society. However, the shift occurs at a much more rapid pace, provoking a break with the past. Some authors have highlighted this sense of discontinuity by referring to DSC as the disintegration of a previous social order or as the break in a frame of reference (Golembiewski et al., 1976 ; Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ; Breakwell and Lyons, 1996 ). They also use terms such as the “construction of something new,” a “reconceptualization,” or a “re-definition.” Indeed, the breakdown of a social structure conveys the need for the reconstruction of core elements in a society. Accordingly, DSC can be conceptualized as a complete rupture in the social structure that marks the end of one period and the beginning of another one, or where a type of society is transformed into another (Tushman and Romanelli, 1985 ; Kohn et al., 2000 ; Weinstein, 2010 ). Other researchers, such as Rogers ( 2003 ), also see rapid social change as intertwined with the social structure. More specifically, Rogers ( 2003 ) states that rapid social change can threaten social structure by surpassing the adaptive capacities of individuals. Unsurprisingly, DSC is the most disruptive type of change not only for the social structure but also for the majority of society members experiencing it, i.e., the normative structure as well as cultural identities are challenged. As DSC entails a re-definition of values, norms and relations, individuals can no longer rely on their habits and routine; they need to learn new skills and new definitions and more challengingly, unlearn the old ways of doing things (Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ; Tomasik et al., 2010 ). Consequently, DSC is described as a painful and confusing experience for individuals (Hinkle, 1952 ; Lapuz, 1976 ; Nadler and Tushman, 1995 ; Kohn et al., 2000 ; Wall and Louchakova, 2002 ; Rioufol, 2004 ; Hegmon et al., 2008 ).

A good example of DSC is the breakdown of the Soviet Union. If I return to Zoia's example, it is clear that all the people in Kyrgyzstan and in the Former Soviet Union were affected by the breakdown of the Soviet Union. Zoia is not the only one who lost all her savings: the vast majority of people lost their savings within a matter of days. In terms of social support, whom could she have relied on if all of her friends were also in the same situation? Regarding to the fall of the former Soviet Union, Goodwin ( 2006 ) argues that older people were inclined to receive less social support in part because the majority of the population, including family members, were struggling with several jobs just to provide themselves with basic needs. Furthermore, elderly citizens could not even rely on formal social services because the collapse of the former Soviet Union caused a decline in formal state support, which left them no time to rebuild their retirement income. This illustrates the rupture in the structure of society that can be found when a DSC occurs as well as the effect on the majority of ordinary group members who cannot rely on collective social support.

Coming full circle: theoretical implications

Heraclitus, an ancient Greek philosopher, is credited for saying that “the only thing constant is change.” Gradually or within an instant, civilizations, societies, communities or organizations that often seem immutable face multiple DSCs. Social scientists agree that social changes are not only intensifying but also defining today's world. In fact, Weinstein ( 2010 ) has underscored that “rapid change, both peaceful and violent, is a fact of life that virtually everyone on Earth today has come to expect, if not unconditionally accept” (p. 3).

For the present paper, my aim was to initiate a conversation about the psychology of social change. Thus, I briefly reviewed the major perspectives of social change in both sociology and psychology. Research conducted in both fields and their subfields have remained in distinct silos with no effort made toward aggregating their findings. This has unfortunately resulted in the absence of an encompassing approach in the current literature of social change: social change has never been integrated into a single perspective that would define or contextualize DSC within the spectrum of different social contexts. More importantly, social change has not been conceptualized so that micro processes, macro processes, and the important relations between them are addressed. As a result, the typology of social change introduces different social contexts (e.g., stability) that can serve as a basis of comparison for DSC. Based on my review of the literature, I suggest four necessary characteristics of DSC (Table ​ (Table6 6 ).

The present paper then offers a first step toward unifying the variety of theories of social change which are currently isolated from each other. Indeed, our approach aims at addressing the challenge raised by Sun and Ryder ( 2016 ) concerning our need for “a more nuanced understanding of rapid sociocultural change combined with sophisticated research methods designed to address change in a multilevel way” (p. 9). The typology of social change I am suggesting is an emerging concept; thus, I invite debate with the hope that the views presented here will stimulate others to contribute to a needed understanding of DSC within an individual perspective. More importantly, based on such a typology of social change, theoretical models could be suggested as they might offer a guide to understanding the consequences of social change. For instance, such theoretical models could answer these three questions: Are the different social contexts associated with one-another? What makes a society move from one social context to another (e.g., from stability to DSC)? What is the role of the different characteristics of DSC? So far, answers to the three questions raised above were left lingering and the different characteristics of DSC were not arranged in a sequential way nor were they identified as key movers of one state of society to another. In Figure ​ Figure1, 1 , I offer a theoretical model that integrates the social contexts and the characteristics of DSC as a first step toward a psychology of social change.

As seen in Figure ​ Figure1, 1 , neither a slow nor a fast pace event will influence the status quo in both stability and inertia. There will therefore be no break with the past and so no rupture in the social and normative structures. Thus, in these two social contexts, if an event were to occur rapidly, the current situation of a group or society would remain unaffected by it; that is why pace is not the only characteristic important to define DSC. For example, if a plane crashes, which is a rapidly occurring dramatic event, it does not necessarily affect an entire community. Also, in a state of stability, when a fast—or slow—event takes place, because the normative and the social structures are unaffected, there is no direct threat to the group's cultural identity. Similarly, when an event occurs in a state of inertia, there is no additional threat to the society's cultural identity, because the normative and social structure are unaffected.

In contrast, in a state of incremental social change, slow-occurring events, if profound enough, will gradually change the social and normative structures, as well as threaten or change cultural identity. For a DSC to occur, a fast event needs to take place. If that event has enough impact—therefore not in a state of stability or inertia—, it will rupture the social structure and the normative structures. As shown by many different DSC contexts, there are three possible scenarios when it comes to the rupture of these two structures: (1) the social structure ruptures first, which later leads to the rupture of the normative structure (e.g., Zhang and Hwang, 2007 ), (2) the normative structure ruptures first, which later leads to the rupture of the social structure (e.g., Centola and Baronchelli, 2015 ), or (3) both the social and normative structures rupture simultaneously and influence each other.

An example of the first scenario would be the latest presidential elections in the United States. The recent proclamation of Donald Trump as president carries the potential for political transformations as well as changes in the United States' economic structure (rupture to social structure). The leadership of Trump's administration can carry major structural change that would then lead to a rupture of the normative structure. At this point, there are indications that this new governance (social structure) may very well affect the normative structure. Some members of the population have become more “open” to expressing their reluctance to have more immigrants come to the USA, which could eventually lead to a rupture in normative structure where different ethnic groups overtly fight each other within America. A second example was the loss of the French Canadians to the English Canadians at the Battle of the Plains of Abraham in 1759. This battle was a pivotal moment in the 7 Years' War and gave power to the British troops (Veyssière, 2013 ). The result of the battle culminated in the French losing most of their economical structural powers to the English and the start of a decline of education. Consequently, the French mentality and behaviors were modified. The norms had to be adapted to new rules and to the loss of economic power (Veyssière, 2013 ).

The normative structure can rupture before the social structure in situations such as the African-American Civil Rights Movement in the United-States, the Fall of Apartheid in South Africa, or the Quiet Revolution in Québec. If in the past African-Americans were afflicted by a sense of resignation, leaders such as Martin Luther King Jr. and Rosa Parks gave them the will they needed to fight for a better future for themselves. This rupture in the normative structure led to the African-American Civil Rights Movement which, in turn, brought about changes to the social structure (e.g., School desegregation). This movement against racial inequality, segregation and discrimination instigated the Civil Rights Act of 1964, which banned any type of segregation based on race, color, religion or sex, as well as other changes in federal legislation.

The breakdown of the Soviet Union is an example that can be used to illustrate a simultaneous rupture of the social and normative structures. This event caused major transformations in the economic, political, and social structures (rupture to social structure). Simultaneously, a large proportion of the population found themselves in a great economic crisis, which led to disruptions in their usual behaviors and habits, such as working multiple jobs instead of just one (rupture of normative structures).

When the normative and the social structures are ruptured (regardless of the order in which this occurs), cultural identity will be threatened. There will be a global sense of confusion, ambiguity, and lack of clarity that might motivate individual group members to change their identification with their group.

Depending on society's and the individual's abilities to cope, there are two possible outcomes: stability or inertia. If the society in which DSC has taken place is able to develop coping and adaptation mechanisms—both at the individual and societal levels—stability might be restored. Stability would then be achieved when the social and normative structures however different are brought back to functionality and when cultural identity is clear and no longer under threat. In contrast, if the society and individuals are not able to develop coping mechanisms, society might enter a state of inertia. In inertia, even though a society in a state of inertia is no longer going through major social changes, the need or desire for change still lingers (Sloutsky and Searle-White, 1993 ). This can be due to a DSC that did not, in the end, really change the way a collectivity is ruled or how its citizens are treated (Moghaddam and Crystal, 1997 ; Moghaddam and Lvina, 2002 ).

Consequences of DSC

Knowing about the range of different social contexts such as stability, inertia, incremental change, and DSC as well as the specific characteristics of DSC, has the potential to guide researchers in terms of assessing DSC and its impact on the psychological well-being of ordinary group members. Specifically, after establishing a clear typology of social change, including potential theoretical models, it is now possible to move on to the second step of the psychology of social change. In this second step, we need to address whether and how different coping mechanisms determine (mediate, moderate) the influence of DSC on psychological well-being. This question goes hand in hand with the work of Norris et al. ( 2002 ) who reviewed 160 studies involving natural disasters, mass violence, and technological disasters. They concluded from more than 60,000 participants that such events have negative repercussions on participants' lives. In most of the research they report, social support, economic status, and age were the identified factors that may be associated with a better adaptation to social change. Although diverse factors were suggested, the research they reported was “atheoretical and little of it is programmatic” (Norris et al., 2002 , p. 249). In accordance with Norris et al. ( 2002 ), I argue that the mediators or moderators involved in adaptation mechanisms should become the focus of future studies. The four characteristics I have identified have the potential to become pivotal in meeting this objective. In sum, the link between social change and well-being is still unclear (e.g., Liu et al., 2014 ; Sun and Ryder, 2016 ). Such an investigation could eventually guide us in designing concrete interventions to help people adapt to the challenges of DSC (Rogers, 2003 ; Vago, 2004 ).

The concept of resilience emerges from the literature as potentially useful for understanding people's coping mechanisms. Resilience is defined as the act of bouncing back in the face of adversity (Bonanno, 2004 ). For the specific example of DSC, resilient individuals would be those who have been able to maintain their normal functioning and adapt themselves to adverse situations (Masten, 2001 ; Curtis and Cicchetti, 2003 ; Luthar, 2003 ; Masten and Powell, 2003 ). Research has shown that a significant number of people are able to adapt to challenging personal situations (e.g., Bonanno, 2004 ). However, resilience has mostly been studied within the context of personal changes such as the death of a loved one or a personal trauma (Bonanno, 2004 ). Similar to a personal change, this variation in reactions may be due to individual differences in resilience. This highlights the need to consider this variable within the psychology of social change. More concretely, the literature on resilience may prove to be important when linking people's perceptions of the characteristic of DSC to the various paths of recovery (e.g., resilience, recovery, chronic distress, and delayed reactions; Bonanno, 2004 ).

While most research on resilience focuses on “personal events,” there is, however, another type of resilience known as “collective resilience” or “community resilience” (e.g., Landau and Saul, 2004 ; Kirmayer et al., 2011 ) which may be more relevant in the context of DSC as the concept hints that the majority of society is affected by the change. To illustrate collective resilience, let us consider the case where the normative structure of a society is dissolved and its cultural identity is threatened. Individuals in this situation would no longer have guidelines and values to individually cope with DSC. Moreover, every individual affected by the change would be in the same negative situation. Consequently, individuals might need to find ways to collectively adapt to the transformations. The processes associated with resilience may thus differ in situations of personal vs. social change. I therefore believe it is important to explore whether the adaptation mechanisms are the same in a context of DSC where social support is not readily available.

Conducting research on social change

In order to speak of a real psychology of social change, we must be able to actually study social change and its consequences. The use of a mix of methodologies that would include large correlational or longitudinal surveys conducted in the field as well as laboratory experiments (de la Sablonnière et al., 2013 ; see also Liu and Bernardo, 2014 ; Sun and Ryder, 2016 ) might prove to be the only way to truly study social change and its consequences. On the one hand, correlational designs conducted in the field are necessary to capture people's firsthand experience with DSC. They are however limited by their design that prevents claims of causality. They are also known to be demanding in terms of both human and financial resources, and may well be dangerous at times for researchers. Moreover, they require an intimate knowledge of the culture such as the language as well as contacts within the community to facilitate the research and collaboration process.

On the other hand, laboratory experiments are necessary to establish the controlled conditions needed to understand associations between the characteristics of social change and the consequences. Laboratory experiments, however, are difficult to design, because it is a challenge to reproduce the actual characteristics of social change in the laboratory which limits their ecological validity (de la Sablonnière et al., 2013 ). Indeed, social change typically entails various elements such as historical processes, a collective perspective, and associated cultural elements (Moghaddam and Crystal, 1997 ) which must be taken into consideration in order to replicate their impact in an artificial setting. For example, the impact of the Tohoku tsunami in Japan or the Syrian conflict cannot be recreated in their entirety in a laboratory; nor can all the characteristic of social change be taken into consideration in a laboratory study designed to assess the impact(s) of social change. However, if an array of studies using different characteristic of DSC were to be conducted (or a combination of multiple characteristic), the convergence of the results would make us able to better understand and thereby predict the impact of DSC on individuals and communities. At the very least in a laboratory, researchers can expose participants to imagined changes through a scenario or a video that would include, in the experimental condition, one or more of the four characteristics of DSC (Pelletier-Dumas et al., submitted). If the scientific community accepts that experimental studies will not exactly mirror DSC, but instead test some of the characteristics in a large number of experiments, there is potential for laboratory experiments to bring an important contribution that would eventually allow a generalization to the real world (for examples see Betsch et al., 2015 ; Caldwell et al., 2016 ; Pelletier-Dumas et al., submitted).

The difficulties of conducting research on social change are, however, amplified by the challenge of obtaining ethical consent in a manner that allows for timely research. In terms of experimental manipulations of DSC, obtaining the ethical board's consent can be tedious. Indeed, according to some authors (Kelman, 1967 ; Bok, 1999 ; Clarke, 1999 ; Herrera, 1999 ; Pittenger, 2002 ) deceiving participants is difficult to justify ethically. This objection on the use of deception can undermine any attempt to seriously study DSC, as deception can be a valuable methodological asset (Bortolotti and Mameli, 2006 ), especially with such an elusive subject. Furthermore, research on new grounds require new techniques and methods on which ethicists can put limits, to ensure that they do not cause harm to participants (Root Wolpe, 2006 ). As with any new technology, methods focused on inducing dramatic-like changes can be perceived as having unsuspected risks.

In order to truly understand the interplay between individuals and their context, social psychological theories must take into account that we live in a constantly changing world. Unfortunately, although social psychology was rooted in understanding social change, most modern psychological theories refrain from addressing a “true” psychology of social change and prefer relegating social change to the field of sociology.

Through increasing the focus on social change, we could combine, on the one hand, sociology's emphasis on the importance of social change with, on the other hand, psychology's emphasis on the importance of complex individual processes. As a result, my theoretical proposal aims at bringing together sociology, where social change is central, and psychology, where rigorous scientific methods allow us to study the psychological processes of individuals living in changing social contexts.

In general, more research on the concept of social change is needed so that we can help predict, prevent, and minimize the negative impact of social change. If psychologists and sociologists work together to move toward developing a psychology of social change, perhaps we could come to better understand and help people, like Zoia, who lost almost everything they had, consequently improving the quality of millions of lives experiencing DSC.

Author contributions

RdlS thought and developed the ideas, as well as wrote the article as sole author. Research assistants were paid to find and read the abstracts of all articles reviewed in this manuscript.

This research was founded by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities research Council of Canada (SSHRC) and by a grant from the Fonds de recherche du Québec – Société et culture (FRQSC).

Conflict of interest statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

RdlS Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal. I wish to thank all my colleagues and the members of the Social Change and Identity Lab for their comments and help. They have heard me talk about social change for the last 10 years and have never stopped encouraging me to pursue these ideas. I am also grateful to all the “Baboushkas” and the people I have met in contexts of DSC. These people continue to inspire me every day. I am grateful to the editor and the three evaluators for their insightful comments. I would also like to thank Matthew Davidson, Saltanat Sadykova, Lily Trudeau-Guévin, Alexie Gendron, Jérémie Dupuis, Raphaël Froment, and Donald M. Taylor for their help during different steps of the preparation of this manuscript. Finally, I want to thank Nada Kadhim who was patient enough to coordinate the material and the team—including me—at all stages.

1 Key sociology readings, such as Human Societies: An Introduction to Macrosociology (Nolan and Lenski, 2011 ), The Sociology of Social Change (Sztompka, 1993 ), and Social Change (Weinstein, 2010 ), offer an in-depth description of these theories that were beyond the scope of the present review.

2 For space limitation, all methodological details and steps I encountered are not discussed here, but are available upon request to the author.

3 The term “social change” must be distinguished from the term “event.” An event may or may not be considered as social change. An event has the potential to bring social change (Sewell, 1996 ), be it incremental or dramatic. However, an event is not always tied to social change as it may represent a form of “happening” that does not influence the course of history (Nisbet, 1972 ). In sum, an event is an intrusion or “disturbances, however mild, of the normal” (p. 26). In contrast to social change, with the event, the disruption of the normal might only be temporary and not significant in time.

4 Defining social structure represents a challenge that goes beyond the scope of the present paper. From my understanding of the literature, there are as many conceptions of social structure as there are scientists working on that concept. The most important issue that demonstrates how hard it is to define social structure is the fact that one of the most prominent sociologists, Giddens ( 1979 ), refers to a “duality of structure” when defining social structure (structure vs. agency). On the one hand, social structure represents institutions or more specifically “collective rules and resources that structure behavior” (Porpora, 1989 , p. 195). Here, scientists refer to “groups, institutions, laws, population characteristics, and set of social relations that form the environment of the organization” (Stinchcombe, 2000 , p. 142), or to “Lawlike regularities that govern the behavior of social facts” (Porpora, 1989 , p. 195). On the other hand, social structure represents “the underlying regularities or patterns in how people behave and in their relationships with one another” (i.e., agency; Giddens et al., 2011 , p. 3). Here, the definitions often described normative behaviors or the roles of individuals rather than the role played by social institutions (e.g., Cortina et al., 2012 ; Homans, 1951 ; Mayhew, 1980 ; Tanner and Jackson, 2012 ; Wilson, 2012 ).

This duality lunched a debate in sociology that was reflected not only in Gidden's work but also in others sociologists that have devoted their writings to defining social structure (e.g., Parsons, 1964 ; Mayhew, 1980 ). For example, Porpora ( 1989 ) reports four principal ways of conceptualizing social structure that reflect either of these conceptions. More recently, expending on the work of Bourdieu ( 1975 ) and of Goffman ( 1983 ), López and Scott ( 2000 ) proposed that there is another aspect of social structure that must also be considered in addition to the institutional and relational structures: the embodied structure described as the “habits and skills that are inscribed in human bodies and minds” (p. 4).

To add to that complexity, some researchers (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, 1979 , 1994 ; for other “system views” see for example Marx, 1859/1970 ; Habermas, 1987 ) describe the possible “systems” that are, like Russian dolls, embedded in each other. These systems include the ecological environments “conceived as a set of nested structures” (Bronfenbrenner, 1994 , p. 39): the microsystems, the mesosystems, the exosystems, the macrosystems, and the chronosystems. This “ecological model” illustrates the complexity of social structure as a sociological term.

Because of the lack of clarity, or maybe because the definition of social structure points to different aspects of the social structure, scientists often avoid defining social structure in their papers, and thereby contribute to the general confusion. Not that the other aspects or levels of social structure are not important (e.g., meso, micro), but the social structure being discussed in the present paper refers exclusively to macro-level elements of society such as institutions and other environmental factors that help facilitate and structure collective interactions, norms, roles, and behaviors.

  • Abrams D., Vasiljevic M. (2014). How does macroeconomic change affect social identity (and vice versa?): insights from the European context . Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 14 , 311–338. 10.1111/asap.12052 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Albert S. (1977). Temporal comparison theory . Psychol. Rev. 84 , 485–503. 10.1037/0033-295X.84.6.485 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Albert S., Sabini J. (1974). Attributions about systems in slow vs. rapid change . Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1 , 91–93. 10.1177/014616727400100131 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Amiot C., de la Sablonnière R., Terry D., Smith J. (2007). Integration of social identities in the self: toward a cognitive-developmental model . Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 11 , 364–388. 10.1177/1088868307304091 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Andersson C., Törnberg A., Törnberg P. (2014). An evolutionary developmental approach to cultural evolution . Curr. Anthropol. 55 , 154–174. 10.1086/675692 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Aoki K., Downes E. J. (2003). An analysis of young people's use of and attitudes toward cell phones . Telematics Inform. 20 , 349–364. 10.1016/S0736-5853(03)00018-2 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Armenakis A. A., Buckley M. R., Bedeian A. G. (1986). Survey research measurement issues in evaluating change: a laboratory investigation . Appl. Psychol. Meas. 10 , 147–157. 10.1177/014662168601000204 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Arnett J. J. (1995). Adolescents' uses of media for self-socialization . J. Youth Adolesc. 24 , 519–533. 10.1007/BF01537054 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ashmore R. D., Deaux K., McLaughlin-Volpe T. (2004). An organizing framework for collective identity: articulation and significance of multidimensionality . Psychol. Bull. 130 , 80–114. 10.1037/0033-2909.130.1.80 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Back K. W. (1971). Biological models of social change . Am. Sociol. Rev. 36 , 660–667. 10.2307/2093596 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bartunek J. M., Moch M. K. (1987). First-order, second-order, and third-order change and organization development interventions: a cognitive approach . J. Appl. Behav. Sci. 23 , 483–500. 10.1177/002188638702300404 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bate S. P. (1994). Strategies for Cultural Change . Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Batel S., Castro P. (2015). Collective action and social change: examining the role of representation in the communication between protesters and third-party members . J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 25 , 249–263. 10.1002/casp.2214 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bauman Z. (2003). Liquid Love: On the Frailty of Human Bonds . Cambridge: Polity Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becker A. E. (2004). Television, disordered eating, and young women in Fiji: negotiating body image and identity during rapid social change . Cult. Med. Psychiatry 28 , 533–559. 10.1007/s11013-004-1067-5 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Benet-Martínez V., Haritatos J. (2005). Bicultural identity integration (BII): components and psychosocial antecedents . J. Pers. 73 , 1015–1050. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00337.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bernstrøm V. H., Kjekshus L. E. (2015). Effect of organisational change type and frequency on long-term sickness absence in hospitals . J. Nurs. Manag. 23 , 813–822. 10.1111/jonm.12218 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berry J. W. (2005). Acculturation: living successfully in two cultures . Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 29 , 697–712. 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.07.013 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bess K. D. (2015). The impact of everyday experiences on planned organizational change: applying schematic change theory to the study of narratives in community-based organizations . J. Community Psychol. 43 , 739–759. 10.1002/jcop.21757 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Betsch T., Lindow S., Engel C., Ulshöfer C., Kleber J. (2015). Has the world changed? My neighbor might know: effects of social context on routine deviation . J. Behav. Decis. Making 28 , 50–66. 10.1002/bdm.1828 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bishop P. (1998). Social change and future practice . Am. Behav. Sci. 42 , 406–412. 10.1177/0002764298042003011 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Blackwood L., Livingstone A. G., Leach C. W. (2013). Regarding societal change . J. Soc. Polit. Psychol. 1 , 105–111. 10.5964/jspp.v1i1.282 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bok S. (1999). Lying: Moral Choice in Public and Private Life . New York, NY: Vintage. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bonanno G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? Am. Psychol. 59 , 20–28. 10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.20 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bortolotti L., Mameli M. (2006). Deception in psychology: mora; costs and benefits of unsought self-knowledge . Account. Res. 13 , 259–275. 10.1080/08989620600848561 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bourdieu P. (1975). Structures sociales et structures de perception du monde social [Social structures and structures of perception of the social world] . Actes Rech. Sci. Soc. 1 , 18–20. 10.3406/arss.1975.3507 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Breakwell G. M. (1986). Coping with Threatened Identities . London: Methuen. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Breakwell G. M., Lyons E. (1996). Changing European Identities: Social Psychological Analyses of Social Change . Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Brignall III T. W., Van Valey T. V. (2005). The impact of Internet communications on social interaction . Sociol. Spectr. 25 , 335–348. 10.1080/02732170590925882 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bronfenbrenner U. (1979). The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bronfenbrenner U. (1994). Ecological models of human development , in International Encyclopedia of Education, 2nd Edn ., Vol. 3 , eds Husén T., Postlethwaite T. N. (Oxford: Elsevier; ), 1643–1647. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bruscella J. (2015). Holiday greeting rituals as expressions of ambivalence and indifference toward social change . West. J. Commun. 79 , 116–132. 10.1080/10570314.2014.943427 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Buchanan D., Fitzgerald L., Ketley D., Gollop R., Jones J. L., Lamont S. S., et al. (2005). No going back: a review of the literature on sustaining organizational change . Int. J. Manag. Rev. 7 , 189–205. 10.1111/j.1468-2370.2005.00111.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Burke W. W., Litwin G. H. (1992). A causal model of organizational performance and change . J. Manag. 18 , 523–545. 10.1177/014920639201800306 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Caldwell C. A., Atkinson M., Renner E. (2016). Experimental approaches to studying cumulative cultural evolution . Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 25 , 191–195. 10.1177/0963721416641049 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Calhoun C. (1992). Social change , in Encyclopedia of Sociology , Vol. 4 , eds Borgatta E. F., Montgomery R. J. V. (New York, NY: MacMillan; ), 1807–1812. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Calhoun C. (2000). Social change , in Encyclopedia of Sociology, 2nd Edn ., Vol. 4 , eds Borgatta E. F., Montgomery R. J. V. (New York, NY: MacMillan; ), 2641–2649. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Centola D., Baronchelli A. (2015). The spontaneous emergence of conventions: an experimental study of structural evolution . Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112 , 1989–1994. 10.1073/pnas.1418838112 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chen X. (2012). Human development in the context of social change: introduction . Child Dev. Perspect. 6 , 321–325. 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00259.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chen X. (2015). Exploring the implications of social change for human development: perspectives, issues and future directions . Int. J. Psychol. 50 , 56–59. 10.1002/ijop.12128 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chirot D., Merton R. M. (1986). Social Change in the Modern Era . San Diego, CA: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Clarke S. (1999). Justifying deception in social science research . J. Appl. Philos. 16 , 151–166. 10.1111/1468-5930.00117 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Comte A. I. (1853/1929). Système de Politique Positive [System of Positive Polity], Vol. 3 . Paris: Carilian-Goeury et Dalmont. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cortina L. M., Curtin N., Stewart A. J. (2012). Where is social structure in personality research? A feminist analysis of publication trends . Psychol. Women Q. 36 , 259–273. 10.1177/0361684312448056 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Coughlin B. J., Khinduka S. K. (1976). Social change and social action . J. Sociol. Soc. Welf. 3 , 322–331. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Crockett L. J., Silbereisen R. K. (Eds.). (2000). Negotiating Adolescence in Times of Social Change . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Curtis W. J., Cicchetti D. (2003). Moving research on resilience into the 21st century: theoretical and methodological considerations in examining the biological contributors to resilience . Dev. Psychopathol. 15 , 773–810. 10.1017/S0954579403000373 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dadrian V. N. (1989). Genocide as a problem of national and international law: the World War I Armenian case and its contemporary legal ramifications . Yale J. Int. Law 12 , 223–326. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dadrian V. N. (1998). The historical and legal interconnections between the Armenian genocide and the Jewish Holocaust: from impunity to retributive justice . Yale J. Int. Law 23 , 503–560. [ Google Scholar ]
  • de la Sablonnière R., Amiot C. E., Cárdenas D., Sadykova N., Gorborukova G. L., Huberdeau M.-E. (2016). Testing the subtractive pattern of cultural identification . Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 46 , 441–454. 10.1002/ejsp.2178 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de la Sablonnière R., Tougas F. (2008). Relative deprivation and social identity in times of dramatic social change: the case of nurses . J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 38 , 2293–2314. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00392.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de la Sablonnière R., Usborne E. (2014). Toward a social psychology of social change: insights from Identity Process Theory , in Identity Process Theory: Identity, Social Action and Social Change , eds Jaspal R., Breakwell G. M. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; ), 203–221. [ Google Scholar ]
  • de la Sablonnière R., Auger É., Sadykova N., Taylor D. M. (2010). When the “we” impacts how “I” feel about myself: effect of temporal collective relative deprivation on personal well-being in the context of dramatic social change in Kyrgyzstan . Eur. Psychol. 15 , 271–282. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000062 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de la Sablonnière R., French Bourgeois L., Najih M. (2013). Dramatic social change: a social psychological perspective . J. Soc. Polit. Psychol. 1 , 253–272. 10.5964/jspp.v1i1.14 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de la Sablonnière R., Taylor D. M., Sadykova N. (2009b). Challenges of applying a student-centered approach to learning in the context of education in Kyrgyzstan . Int. J. Educ. Dev. 29 , 628–634. 10.1016/j.ijedudev.2009.01.001 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de la Sablonnière R., Taylor D. M., Perozzo C., Sadykova N. (2009a). Reconceptualizing relative deprivation in the context of dramatic social change: the challenge confronting the people of Kyrgyzstan . Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 39 , 325–345. 10.1002/ejsp.519 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de la Sablonnière R., Tougas F., Lortie-Lussier M. (2009c). Dramatic social change in Russia and Mongolia: connecting relative deprivation to social identity . J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 40 , 327–348. 10.1177/0022022108330986 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • de Lemus S., Stroebe K. (2015). Achieving social change: a matter of all for one? J. Soc. Issues 71 , 441–452. 10.1111/josi.12122 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Delanty G. (2012). A cosmopolitan approach to the explanation of social change: social mechanisms, processes, modernity . Sociol. Rev. 60 , 333–354. 10.1111/j.1467-954X.2012.02076.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Des Forges A. (1999). Leave None to Tell the Story: Genocide in Rwanda . New York, NY: Human Rights Watch; Available online at: https://www.hrw.org/reports/pdfs/r/rwanda/rwanda993.pdf [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diamond J. (2005). Collapse: How Societies Choose to Fail or Succeed . New York, NY: Penguin Books. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Diekman A. B., Goodfriend W. (2007). The good and bad of social change: ambivalence toward activist groups . Soc. Justice Res. 20 , 401–417. 10.1007/s11211-007-0050-z [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • DiMaggio P., Hargittai E., Neuman W. R., Robinson J. P. (2001). Social implications of the Internet . Annu. Rev. Sociol. 27 , 307–336. 10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.307 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Durkheim É. (1893/1967). De la Division du Travail Social [The Division of Labor in Society] . Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Durkheim É. (1897/1967). Le Suicide: Étude de Sociologie [Suicide: A Study in Sociology]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ethier K. A., Deaux K. (1994). Negotiating social identity when contexts change: maintaining identification and responding to threat . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67 , 243–251. 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.243 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Feldman M. W., Laland K. N. (1996). Gene-culture coevolutionary theory . Trends Ecol. Evol. 11 , 453–457. 10.1016/0169-5347(96)10052-5 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fried M. (1964). Effects of social change on mental health . Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 34 , 3–28. 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1964.tb02187.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fukuyama F. (1999). The Great Disruption: Human Nature and the Reconstitution of Social Order . New York, NY: Free Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Giddens A. (1979). Central Problems in Social Theory: Action, Structure, and Contradiction in Social Analysis . Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Giddens A., Duneier M., Appelbaum R. P., Carr D. (2011). Introduction to Sociology, 8th Edn . New York, NY: Norton Publishing. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goffman E. (1983). The interaction order . Am. Sociol. Rev. 48 , 1–17. 10.2307/2095141 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goldin C., Katz L. F. (2002). The power of the pill: oral contraceptives and women's career and marriage decisions . J. Polit. Econ. 110 , 730–770. 10.1086/340778 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Golembiewski R. T., Billingsley K., Yeager S. (1976). Measuring change and persistence in human affairs: types of change generated by OD designs . J. Appl. Behav. Sci. 12 , 133–157. 10.1177/002188637601200201 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goodwin R. (2006). Age and social support perception in Eastern Europe: social change and support in four rapidly changing countries . Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 45 , 799–815. 10.1348/014466605X72144 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goodwin R. B. (1998). Personal relationships and social change: the ‘Realpolitik’ of cross-cultural research in transient cultures . J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 15 , 227–247. 10.1177/0265407598152006 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greenfield P. M. (2009). Linking social change and developmental change: shifting pathways of human development . Dev. Psychol. 45 , 401–418. 10.1037/a0014726 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Greenfield P. M. (2016). Social change, cultural evolution, and human development . Curr. Opin. Psychol. 8 , 84–92. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.012 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Habermas H. (1987). The Theory of Communicative Action, 3rd Edn., Vol. 2 . Transl. by McCarthy T. A. Boston, MA: Beacon Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hannum H. (1989). International law and Cambodian genocide: the sounds of silence . Hum. Rights Q. 11 , 82–138. 10.2307/761936 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hansen N., Postmes T., van der Vinne N., van Thiel W. (2012). Information and communication technology and cultural change . Soc. Psychol. 43 , 222–231. 10.1027/1864-9335/a000123 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Harmon D. J., Green S. E., Goodnight G. T. (2015). A model of rhetorical legitimation: the structure of communication and cognition underlying institutional maintenance and change . Acad. Manag. Rev. 40 , 76–95. 10.5465/amr.2013.0310 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hegmon M., Peeples M. A., Kinzig A. P., Kulow S., Meegan C. M., Nelson M. C. (2008). Social transformation and its human costs in the prehispanic U.S . Southwest. Am. Anthropol. 110 , 313–324. 10.1111/j.1548-1433.2008.00041.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Herrera C. (1999). Two arguments for covert methods in social research . Br. J. Sociol. 50 , 331–343. 10.1080/000713199358770 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hewitt W. E., White J., Teevan J. J. (2008). Introduction to Sociology: A Canadian Focus, 8th Edn . Toronto, ON: Pearson Prentice Hall. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hinkle G. J. (1952). The “four wishes” in Thomas' theory of social change . Soc. Res. 19 , 464–484. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hoffman M. F., Medlock-Klyukovski A. (2004). “Our creator who art in heaven”: paradox, ritual, and cultural transformation . West. J. Commun. 68 , 389–410. 10.1080/10570310409374810 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Homans C. G. (1951). The Human Group . London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jerneić Ž., Šverko B. (2001). Life-role changes in times of socioeconomic transition . Rev. Psychol. 8 , 41–48. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jost J. T., Banaji M. R., Nosek B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo . Polit. Psychol. 25 , 881–919. 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kanter R. M. (1991). Transcending business boundaries: 12,000 world managers view change . Harv. Bus. Rev. 69 , 151–164. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Katz D. (1974). Factors affecting social change: a social-psychological interpretation . J. Soc. Issues 30 , 159–180. 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1974.tb00732.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kelman H. (1967). Humans use of human subjects: the problem of deception in social psychological experiments . Psychol. Bull. 67 , 1–11. 10.1037/h0024072 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kiesler S., Siegel J., McGuire T. W. (1984). Social psychological aspects of computer-mediated communication . Am. Psychol. 39 , 1123–1134. 10.1037/0003-066X.39.10.1123 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kim J. (2008). Perception of social change and psychological well-being: a study focusing on social change in Korea between 1997 and 2000 . J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 38 , 2821–2858. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00415.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kim J., Ng S. H. (2008). Perceptions of social changes and social identity: study focusing on Hong Kong society after reunification . Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 11 , 232–240. 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2008.00262.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kirmayer L. J., Dandeneau S., Marshall E., Phillips M. K., Williamson K. J. (2011). Rethinking resilience from indigenous perspectives . Can. J. Psychiatry 56 , 84–91. 10.1177/070674371105600203 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kohn M. L., Slomczynski K. M., Janicka K., Khmelko V., Mach B. W., Paniotto V., et al. (1997). Social structure and personality under conditions of radical social change: a comparative analysis of Poland and Ukraine . Am. Sociol. Rev. 62 , 614–638. 10.2307/2657430 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kohn M. L., Zaborowski W., Janicka K., Mach B. W., Khmelko V., Slomczynski K. M., et al. (2000). Complexity of activities and personality under conditions of radical social change: a comparative analysis of Poland and Ukraine . Soc. Psychol. Q. 63 , 187–207. 10.2307/2695868 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kollontai V. (1999). Social transformations in Russia . Int. Soc. Sci. J. 51 , 103–121. 10.1111/1468-2451.00180 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Krznaric R. (2007). How Change Happens: Interdisciplinary Perspectives for Human Development . Oxford: Oxfam. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Laland K. N., Odling-Smee J., Feldman M. W. (2000). Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change . Behav. Brain Sci. 23 , 131–175. 10.1017/S0140525X00002417 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Landau J., Saul J. (2004). Facilitating family and community resilience in response to major disaster , in Living Beyond Loss, 2nd Edn. , eds Walsh F., McGoldrick M. (New York, NY: Norton; ), 285–309. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lapuz L. V. (1976). Culture change and psychological stress . Am. J. Psychoanal. 36 , 171–176. 10.1007/BF01248367 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Latour B. (2005). Reassembling the Social: An Introduction to Actor-Network-Theory . New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lenski G. E., Lenski J. (1974). Human Societies . New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liu H., Li S., Xiao Q., Feldman M. W. (2014). Social support and psychological well-being under social change in urban and rural China . Soc. Indic. Res. 119 , 979–996. 10.1007/s11205-013-0534-1 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liu J. H., Bernardo A. B. I. (2014). Social psychology for social change: foundations for and introduction to a program of action-oriented research . J. Pacific Rim Psychol. 8 , 29–34. 10.1017/prp.2014.4 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • López J., Scott J. (2000). Social Structure . Maidenhead: Open University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Luthar S. S. (2003). Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities . New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Macek P., Ježek S., Vazsonyi A. T. (2013). Adolescents during and after times of social change: the case of the Czech Republic . J. Early Adolesc. 33 , 1029–1047. 10.1177/0272431613507758 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Macionis J. J., Jansson M., Benoit C. M. (2008). Society: The Basics, 4th Edn. Toronto, ON: Pearson Education. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marquis D. G. (1947). Psychology of social change . Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 249 , 75–80. 10.1177/000271624724900110 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marx K. (1859/1970). A Contribution to the Critique of Political Economy . New York, NY: International Publishers. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Marx K., Engels F. (1848). Manifesto of the Communist Party, Vol. 1 . Moscow: Progress Publishers. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Masten A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: resilience processes in development . Am. Psychol. 56 , 227–238. 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.227 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Masten A. S., Powell J. L. (2003). A resilience framework for research, policy, and practice , in Resilience and Vulnerability: Adaptation in the Context of Childhood Adversities , ed Luthar S. S. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; ), 1–25. [ Google Scholar ]
  • May V. (2011). Self, belonging and social change . Sociology 45 , 363–378. 10.1177/0038038511399624 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mayhew B. H. (1980). Structuralism versus individualism: part 1, shadowboxing in the dark . Soc. Forces 59 , 335–375. 10.2307/2578025 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McDade T. W., Worthman C. M. (2004). Socialization ambiguity in Samoan adolescents: a model for human development and stress in the context of culture change . J. Res. Adolesc. 14 , 49–72. 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2004.01401003.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • McGrath J. E. (1983). Looking ahead by looking backwards: some recurrent themes about social change . J. Soc. Issues 39 , 225–239. 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1983.tb00186.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Meyer J., Allen N. (1997). Commitment in the Workplace: Theory, Research and Application . New York, NY: Sage publications. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moghaddam F. M. (2002). The Individual and Society: A Cultural Integration . New York, NY: Worth. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moghaddam F. M. (2012). The omnicultural imperative . Cult. Psychol. 18 , 304–330. 10.1177/1354067X12446230 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moghaddam F. M., Crystal D. S. (1997). Revolutions, samurai, and reductions: the paradoxes of change and continuity in Iran and Japan . Polit. Psychol. 18 , 355–384. 10.1111/0162-895X.00061 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Moghaddam F. M., Lvina E. (2002). Toward a psychology of societal change and stability: the case of human rights and duties . Int. J. Group Tens. 31 , 31–51. 10.1023/A:1014212700468 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mucchi-Faina A., Pacilli M. G., Pagliaro S. (2010). Minority influence, social change, and social stability . Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 4 , 1111–1123. 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00314.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nadler D. A., Tushman M. L. (1995). Types of organizational change: from incremental improvement to discontinuous transformation , in Discontinuous Change: Leading Organizational Transformation , eds Nadler D. A., Shaw R. B., Walton A. E. (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; ), 14–33. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Neves P., Caetano A. (2009). Commitment to change: contributions to trust in the supervisor and work outcomes . Group Organ. Manag. 34 , 623–644. 10.1177/1059601109350980 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Newman K. L. (2000). Organizational transformation during institutional upheaval . Acad. Manag. Rev. 25 , 602–619. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nisbet R. A. (1972). Social Change . New York, NY: Harper and Row. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Noak P., Kracke B., Wild E., Hofer M. (2001). Subjective experiences of social change in East and West Germany: analyses of the perceptions of adolescents and their parents . Am. Behav. Sci. 44 , 1798–1817. 10.1177/00027640121958168 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Nolan P., Lenski G. E. (2011). Human Societies: An Introduction to Macrosociology, 11th Edn . Boulder, CO: Paradigm. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Norris F. H., Friedman M. J., Watson P. J., Byrne C. M., Diaz E., Kaniasty K. (2002). 60,000 disaster victims speak: part I. An empirical review of the empirical literature, 1981-2001 . Psychiatry 65 , 207–239. 10.1521/psyc.65.3.207.20173 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • O'Keeffe G. S., Clarke-Pearson K. (2011). The impact of social media on children, adult, and families . Am. Acad. Pediatr. 127 , 800–806. 10.1542/peds.2011-0054 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Oh O., Eom C., Rao H. R. (2015). Role of social media in social change: an analysis of collective sense making during the 2011 Egypt revolution . Inform. Syst. Res. 26 , 210–223. 10.1287/isre.2015.0565 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pareto V. (1901/1968). The Rise and Fall of Elites: An Application of Theoretical Sociology . Towota, NJ: Bedminster Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Parson T. (1951). The Social Structure of Social Action . New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Parsons T. (1964). Essays in Sociological Theory . New York, NY: Free Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pham P. N., Weinstein H. M., Longman T. (2004). Trauma and PTSD symptoms in Rwanda . J. Am. Med. Assoc. 292 , 602–612. 10.1001/jama.292.5.602 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pinquart M., Silbereisen R. K. (2004). Human development in times of social change: theoretical considerations and research needs . Int. J. Behav. Dev. 28 , 289–298. 10.1080/01650250344000406 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pinquart M., Silbereisen R. K., Juang L. P. (2004). Moderating effects of adolescents' self-efficacy beliefs on psychological responses to social change . J. Adolesc. Res. 19 , 340–359. 10.1177/0743558403258851 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pinquart M., Silbereisen R. K., Körner A. (2009). Perceived work-related demands associated with social change, control strategies, and psychological well-being: do associations vary by regional economic conditions? Evidence from Germany . Eur. Psychol. 14 , 207–219. 10.1027/1016-9040.14.3.207 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pittenger D. J. (2002). Deception in research: distinctions and solutions from the perspective of utilitarianism . Ethics Behav. 12 , 117–142. 10.1207/S15327019EB1202_1 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Pizer S. A., Travers J. R. (1975). Psychology and Social Change . New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ponsioen J. A. (1962). The Analysis of Social Change Reconsidered: A Sociological Study . The Hague: Mouton. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Porpora D. V. (1989). Four concepts of social structure . J. Theory Soc. Behav. 19 , 195–211. 10.1111/j.1468-5914.1989.tb00144.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Prilleltensky I. (1990). Enhancing the social ethics of psychology: toward a psychology at the service of social change . Can. Psychol. 31 , 310–319. 10.1037/h0078954 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Prislin R., Christensen P. N. (2005). Social change in the aftermath of successful minority influence . Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 16 , 43–73. 10.1080/10463280440000071 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Prunier G. (2010). Africa's World War: Congo, the Rwandan Genocide, and the Making of a Continental Catastrophe . Oxford: Oxford University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Reichers A. E., John P. W., James T. A. (1997). Understanding and managing cynicism about organizational change . Acad. Manag. Exec. 11 , 48–59. 10.5465/ame.1997.9707100659 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rieger J. (2003). A retrospective visual study of social change: the pulp-logging industry in an Upper Peninsula Michigan county . Vis. Stud. 18 , 157–178. 10.1080/14725860310001632010 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rioufol V. (2004). Approaches to social change in social forums: snapshots of recompositions in progress . Int. Soc. Sci. J. 56 , 551–563. 10.1111/j.0020-8701.2004.00516.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Robinson J. P., Barth K., Kohut A. (1997). Social impact research: personal computers, mass media, and use of time . Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 15 , 65–82. 10.1177/089443939701500107 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rocher G. (1992). Introduction à la Sociologie Générale [General Introduction to Sociology] . Montréal, QC: Éditions Hurtubise. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rodriguez S. (2013). Making sense of social change: observing collective action in networked cultures . Sociol. Compass 7 , 1053–1064. 10.1111/soc4.12088 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rogers E. M. (2003). Diffusion of Innovations, 5th Edn . New York, NY: Free Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Root Wolpe P. (2006). Reasons scientists avoid thinking about ethics . Cell 125 , 1023–1025. 10.1016/j.cell.2006.06.001 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Round J., Williams C. (2010). Coping with the social costs of “transition”: everyday life in post-Soviet Russia and Ukraine . Eur. Urban Reg. Stud. 17 , 183–196. 10.1177/0969776409356158 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rudel T. K., Hooper L. (2005). Is the pace of social change accelerating? Latecomers, common languages, and rapid historical declines in fertility . Int. J. Comp. Sociol. 46 , 275–296. 10.1177/0020715205059204 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Runciman W. G. (1966). Relative Deprivation and Social Justice: A Study of Attitudes to Social Inequality in Twentieth-Century England . Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sampson E. E. (1989). The challenge of social change for psychology: globalization and psychology's theory of the person . Am. Psychol. 44 , 914–921. 10.1037/0003-066X.44.6.914 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sanzgiri J., Gottlieb J. Z. (1992). Philosophic and pragmatic influences on the practice of organization development, 1950-2000 . Organiz. Dyn. 21 , 57–69. 10.1016/0090-2616(92)90064-T [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Saran A. K. (1963). The Marxian theory of social change . Inquiry 6 , 70–128. 10.1080/00201746308601368 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schaal S., Elbert T. (2006). Ten years after the genocide: trauma confrontation and posttraumatic stress in Rwandan adolescents . J. Trauma Stress 19 , 95–105. 10.1002/jts.20104 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schneiderman L. (1988). The Psychology of Social Change . Ann Arbor, MI: Human Sciences Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Schrickel H. G. (1945). Group conflict and social reform . J. Soc. Psychol. 21 , 187–196. 10.1080/00224545.1945.9714165 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sewell W. H. (1996). Three temporalities: toward an eventful sociology , in The Historic Turn in the Human Sciences , ed McDonald T. J. (Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press; ), 245–280. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sidanius J., Pratto F. (1999). Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Silbereisen R. K., Tomasik M. J. (2010). Human behavior in response to social change: a guide to the special section . Eur. Psychol. 15 , 243–245. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000059 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Slone M., Kaminer D., Durrheim K. (2002). Appraisal of sociopolitical change among South African youth: the relation to psychological maladjustment . J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 32 , 318–341. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb00218.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sloutsky V. M., Searle-White J. (1993). Psychological responses of Russians to rapid social change in the former USSR . Polit. Psychol. 14 , 511–526. 10.2307/3791710 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smelser N. J., Swedberg R. (1994). The sociological perspective on the economy , in The Handbook of Economic Sociology , eds Smelser N. J., Swedberg R. (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; ), 3–26. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Smith A. D. (1973). The Concept of Social Change: A Critique of the Functionalist Theory of Social Change . London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Spencer H. (1898). The Principles of Sociology , Vol. 1 New York, NY: Appleton and Company. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Staub E., Pearlman L. A., Gubin A., Hagengimana A. (2005). Healing, reconciliation, forgiving and the prevention of violence after genocide or mass killing: an intervention and its experimental evaluation in Rwanda . J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 24 , 297–334. 10.1521/jscp.24.3.297.65617 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Steele C. M., Spencer S. J., Aronson J. (2002). Contending with group image: the psychology of stereotype and social identity threat . Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 34 , 379–440. 10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80009-0 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stinchcombe A. L. (2000). Social structure and organizations , in Economics Meets Sociology in Strategic Management (Advances in Strategic Management) , Vol. 17 , eds Baum J., Dobbin F. (Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited; ), 229–259. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Stroebe K., Wang K., Wright S. C. (2015). Broadening perspectives on achieving social change . J. Soc. Issues 71 , 633–645. 10.1111/josi.12132 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Subašić E., Reynolds K. J., Turner J. C. (2008). The political solidarity model of social change: dynamics of self-categorization in intergroup power relations . Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 12 , 330–352. 10.1177/1088868308323223 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Subašić E., Reynolds K. J., Reicher S. D., Klandermans B. (2012). Where to from here for the psychology of social change? Future directions for theory and practice . Polit. Psychol. 33 , 61–74. 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00864.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sun J., Ryder A. G. (2016). The Chinese experience of rapid modernization: sociocultural changes, psychological consequences? Front. Psychol. 7 :477. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00477 [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sztompka P. (1993). The Sociology of Social Change . Oxford: Blackwell. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sztompka P. (1998). Devenir social, néo-modernisation et importance de la culture: quelques implications de la révolution anticommuniste pour la théorie du changement social [To become social, neo-modernization and importance of culture: a few implications of the anticommunist revolution for social change theory] . Sociol. Soc. 30 , 85–94. 10.7202/001781ar [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sztompka P. (2000). Cultural trauma: the other face of social change . Eur. J. Soc. Theory 3 , 449–466. 10.1177/136843100003004004 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sztompka P. (2004). The trauma of social change: a case of postcommunist societies , in Cultural Trauma and Collective Identity , eds Alexander J. C., Eyerman R., Giesen B., Smelser N. J., Sztompka P. (Berkeley, CA: University of California Press; ), 155–195. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tajfel H., Turner J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behaviour , in Psychology of Intergroup Relations , eds Worchel S., Austin W. G. (Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall; ), 7–24. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tanner C. J., Jackson A. L. (2012). Social structure emerges via the interaction between local ecology and individual behaviour . J. Anim. Ecol. 81 , 260–267. 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2011.01879.x [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor D. M. (1997). The quest for collective identity: the plight of disadvantaged ethnic minorities . Can. Psychol. 38 , 174–190. 10.1037/0708-5591.38.3.174 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor D. M. (2002). The Quest for Identity: From Minority Groups to Generation Xers . Westport, CT: Greenwood. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor D. M., de la Sablonnière R. (2013). Why interventions in dysfunctional communities fail: the need for a truly collective approach . Can. Psychol. 54 , 22–29. 10.1037/a0031124 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor D. M., de la Sablonnière R. (2014). Towards Constructive Change in Aboriginal Communities: A Social Psychology Perspective . Montréal, QC: McGill-Queen's Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Taylor D. M., McKirnan D. J. (1984). Theoretical contributions: a five-stage model of intergroup relations . Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 23 , 291–300. 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1984.tb00644.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Terry D. J., Jimmieson N. L. (2003). A stress and coping approach to organisational change: evidence from three field studies . Aust. Psychol. 38 , 92–101. 10.1080/00050060310001707097 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thomas D. R., Veno A. (1992). Psychology and Social Change: Creating an International Agenda . Palmerston North: Dunmore Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thompson R. C., Hunt J. G. (1996). Inside the black box of alpha, beta, and gamma change: using a cognitive-processing model to assess attitude structure . Acad. Manag. Rev. 21 , 655–690. 10.2307/258998 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tilly C., Tilly L. A., Tilly R. H. (1975). The Rebellious Century: 1830-1930 . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tomasik M. J., Silbereisen R. K., Pinquart M. (2010). Individuals negotiating demands of social and economic change: a control-theoretical approach . Eur. Psychol. 15 , 246–259. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000064 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tonkens E. (2012). Working with Arlie Hochschild: connecting feelings to social change . Soc. Polit. 19 , 194–218. 10.1093/sp/jxs003 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tushman M. L., Romanelli E. (1985). Organizational evolution: a metamorphosis model of convergence and reorientation . Res. Organ. Behav. 7 , 171–222. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Usborne E., de la Sablonnière R. (2014). Understanding my culture means understanding myself: the function of cultural identity clarity for personal identity clarity and personal psychological well-being . J. Theory Soc. Behav. 44 , 436–458. 10.1111/jtsb.12061 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Usborne E., Taylor D. M. (2010). The role of cultural identity clarity for self-concept clarity, self-esteem, and subjective well-being . Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36 , 883–897. 10.1177/0146167210372215 [ PubMed ] [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vago S. (2004). Social Change, 5th Edn . Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Binh T. (2002). Social and cultural changes in Vietnam with the new market economy . Nat. Soc. Thought 15 , 335–346. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Varnum M. E. W. (2008). Rapid adaptation to social change in Central Europe: changes in locus of control, attribution, subjective well-being, self-direction, and trust . Sociológia 40 , 215–235. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vaughan G. M. (1986). Social change and racial identity: issues in the use of picture and doll measures . Aust. J. Psychol. 38 , 359–370. 10.1080/00049538608259022 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Veyssière L. (2013). La Nouvelle-France en Héritage [New France in Heritage] . Paris: Armand Colin. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Walker C., Stephenson S. (2010). Youth and social change in Eastern Europe and the former Soviet Union . J. Youth Stud. 13 , 521–532. 10.1080/13676261.2010.487522 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wall K., Louchakova O. (2002). Evolution of consciousness in response to terrorist attacks: towards a transpersonal theory of cultural transformation . Humanist. Psychol. 30 , 252–273. 10.1080/08873267.2002.9977040 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Watzlawick P., Weakland J. H., Fisch R. (1974). Change: Principles of Problem Formation and Problem Resolution . New York, NY: Norton. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Weinstein J. (2010). Social Change, 3rd Edn . Lanham, MD: Roman and Littlefield. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wilson W. J. (2012). The Truly Disadvantaged: The Inner City, The Underclass, and Public Policy . Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Wyn J., White R. (2000). Negotiating social change: the paradox of youth . Youth Soc. 32 , 165–183. 10.1177/0044118X00032002002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yakushko O. (2008). The impact of social and political changes on survivors of political persecutions in rural Russia and Ukraine . Polit. Psychol. 29 , 119–130. 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2007.00615.x [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Yanagizawa-Drott D. (2014). Propaganda and conflict: evidence from the Rwandan genocide . Q. J. Econ. 129 , 1947–1994. 10.1093/qje/qju020 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang X., Hwang S.-S. (2007). The micro consequences of macro-level social transition: how did Russians survive in the 1990s? Soc. Indic. Res. 82 , 337–360. 10.1007/s11205-006-9037-7 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zorbas E. (2004). Reconciliation in post-genocide Rwanda . Afr. J. Legal Stud. 1 , 29–52. 10.1163/221097312X13397499735904 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zuck A. M. (1997). Introduction . Am. Behav. Sci. 40 , 257–258. 10.1177/0002764297040003002 [ CrossRef ] [ Google Scholar ]

IMAGES

  1. Social Change Research Project by Sarah at Lit Think

    how can a research project cause social change

  2. (PDF) "Social Change Project"

    how can a research project cause social change

  3. Social Change: Sources and Issues Free Essay Example

    how can a research project cause social change

  4. PPT

    how can a research project cause social change

  5. (PDF) Social Change

    how can a research project cause social change

  6. PPT

    how can a research project cause social change

VIDEO

  1. Three Minute Sociologist:

  2. Social Structural change in Sociology

  3. Socio Economic Perspective of Climate Change

  4. 3.1.3 Factors responsible for Social change//Biological , physical , Technological , Cultural

  5. Designing for Social Change: Andrew Shea at TEDxTransmedia

  6. How Change Happens

COMMENTS

  1. Transformative Research Methods to Increase Social Impact for

    An example of political pressure that drives researchers to demonstrate impact arose when the European Commission (EU) (2013) questioned the usefulness of social science and humanities research because the impact of the research for improved social conditions was not systematically documented. In response to this challenge, the EU funded the IMPACT-EV initiative that was led by a team of ...

  2. Research that results in positive social change

    The effective translation and dissemination of research can help bridge the gap between research findings and their application to policy and practice and hence have a positive social impact. Journals play a key part in this process and where the peer review process helps maintain high standards and credibility (Reupert, Citation 2021). Journal ...

  3. Approaching the Social Impact of Research Through a Literature Review

    The social impact of research has been considered a change in behaviour motivated by a previous research effort (Esko & Miettinen, 2019; Spaapen & Van Drooge, 2011).This research has, for example, led to the development of new applications and solutions that solve existing societal problems (Spaapen et al., 2011).It is the use of the research result that generates a benefit or influence (Lima ...

  4. The Role of Social Research in Effective Social Change Programs

    Social research is a critical foundation for programs that seek to engage communities in change and in the development of more sustainable societies. Without appropriate research, programs aimed at change are likely to be based on implicit or assumed problem identification and/or inferred community needs and wishes.

  5. Contributions of Social Research Methodologies to Social Change: Giving

    In order to understand what social methodologies can contribute to social change, we used the theory of social change as espoused by de la Sablonniere (2017). De la Sablonniere (2017) purports that social change can be categorised into four contexts. They are stability, inertia, incremental social change and dramatic social change.

  6. Social change

    Social change can evolve from a number of different sources, including contact with other societies (diffusion), changes in the ecosystem (which can cause the loss of natural resources or widespread disease), technological change (epitomized by the Industrial Revolution, which created a new social group, the urban proletariat), and population ...

  7. Social change

    Social change - Causes, Effects, Processes: One way of explaining social change is to show causal connections between two or more processes. This may take the form of determinism or reductionism, both of which tend to explain social change by reducing it to one supposed autonomous and all-determining causal process. A more cautious assumption is that one process has relative causal priority ...

  8. Re-thinking the conditions for social change and innovation

    This Open Issue of Innovation brings together articles that seem to share the view that social innovation needs to be conceptualised in relation to social change more broadly by considering the context of innovation and the relationship between the micro level and emerging macro-level structures, such as welfare reform (Cools et al.; Johansson ...

  9. 5 Things That Everyone Should Know About Social Change

    Walden devotes itself to making social change possible by providing a diverse community of career professionals with the opportunity to transform themselves as scholar-practitioners so that they can apply what they've learned to effect positive social change. The list of social change projects led by Walden students and graduates includes a ...

  10. Toward a Psychology of Social Change: A Typology of Social Change

    The Typology of Social Change. In order to properly conceptualize DSC and other social contexts associated with the state of a collectivity, I suggest a typology of social change comprised of four different social contexts: "stability," "inertia," "incremental social change," and "DSC" (see Table 1 for definitions).

  11. PDF How Participatory Action Research Can Promote Social Change and Help

    Youth Participatory Action Research (YPAR) is a tool for increasing youth involvement in social movement organizing that can generate renewed enthusiasm for social change and create new opportunities for youth leadership. Educators, philanthropists, activists, and community leaders (young and old) work together on all PAR projects.

  12. Causes of Social Change

    Steam and rail power were the guiding forces of social change and globalization in this period. Finally, Friedman brings us to the post-millennial era. In this period of globalization, change is driven by technology, particularly the Internet (Friedman 2005). But also consider that technology can create change in the other three forces social ...

  13. Causes of Social Change

    Steam and rail power were the guiding forces of social change and globalization in this period. Finally, Friedman brings us to the post-millennial era. In this period of globalization, change is driven by technology, particularly the Internet (Friedman 2005). But also consider that technology can create change in the other three forces social ...

  14. Social Change: Definition, Characteristics, Causes, Types, and Examples

    According to sociologists, social change is a constantly occurring phenomenon. It is the process through which social structures and institutions are reconstructed, undergoing a cultural transformation. Society is built upon certain value systems that maintain social order and shifts in the root of these value systems- human interaction- lead ...

  15. Are we living in a time of particularly rapid social change? And how

    We consider the problems associated with measuring social change. The paper presents data on social change in the US over the last century or so. We find that Americans are not living in a time of particularly rapid social change. The data does not support the notion that change occurs in long wave-like cycles.

  16. (PDF) Empirical Studies of Law and Social Change: What Is the Field

    2013:171 Empirical Studies of Law and Social Change 175. critical legal studies, critical race theory, and clinical theory), the. empirical study of law's relationship to social activism, its ...

  17. How Participatory Action Research Can Promote Social Change and Help

    Youth Participatory Action Research (YPAR) is a tool for increasing youth involvement. in social movement organizing that can generate renewed enthusiasm for social change and. create new ...

  18. Full article: Bringing ethical thinking to social change initiatives

    As new issues emerge, returning to project values can be helpful during deliberations of how to support/mitigate in ethically-considered ways the new effects of social change processes. Defining a shared set of values can guide what decisions are made vis-a-vis emerging social changes throughout the NSI implementation cycle.

  19. Addressing Trauma as a Pathway to Social Change

    In addition to undergirding our future research, we hope these insights can offer thought-provoking framing for current social change leaders. 1. Finding root causes and compounding factors. Recognizing intergenerational trauma can help spotlight and define the roots of social change issues. Consider the high costs of poverty in the United ...

  20. Toward a Psychology of Social Change: A Typology of Social Change

    First, SIT is a theory of social structure that is based on perceptions of legitimacy, stability, and permeability. Second, SIT proposes identity management strategies such as collective action whereby minority groups aim to maintain or acquire a positive and distinctive social identity. Tajfel and Turner, 1986.

  21. The Potential of Music to Effect Social Change

    The 18th-century debate on the roles of music and language as tools for social interaction has not ceased and has been continued by other scholars. According to Steven Brown (2000), for example, music and language may have evolved from a stage called 'musilanguage', sharing ancestral features of music and language.

  22. 4 Small Ways to Make a Big Social Change Impact

    Practice Random Acts of Kindness. Small, random acts of kindness—like smiling at a stranger or holding the door open for someone—can be a great way to make a social change impact. Sometimes these random acts of kindness even start a ripple effect that can spread far beyond the first actor. 1 The great thing is that it doesn't take much ...