U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Here's how you know

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Home

Alcohol Research: Current Reviews (ARCR)

ARCR, a peer-reviewed scientific journal published by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism at the National Institutes of Health, marks its 50th anniversary in 2024. Explore our "News & Notes" webpage for more on this historic accomplishment.

Recent Articles

ORCID logo

Liz Simon, Brianna L. Bourgeois, and Patricia E. Molina

News and Notes

50th years of insights into alcohol research

25 January 2024

ARCR Celebrates Its 50th Anniversary

2024 marks the 50th anniversary of Alcohol Research: Current Reviews (ARCR) , an open-access, peer-reviewed journal published by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) at the National Institutes of Health.

Substance Use Disorders and Addiction: Mechanisms, Trends, and Treatment Implications

Information & authors, metrics & citations, view options, insights into mechanisms related to cocaine addiction using a novel imaging method for dopamine neurons, treatment implications of understanding brain function during early abstinence in patients with alcohol use disorder, relatively low amounts of alcohol intake during pregnancy are associated with subtle neurodevelopmental effects in preadolescent offspring, increased comorbidity between substance use and psychiatric disorders in sexual identity minorities, trends in nicotine use and dependence from 2001–2002 to 2012–2013, conclusions, information, published in.

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry

  • Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders
  • Addiction Psychiatry
  • Transgender (LGBT) Issues

Competing Interests

Export citations.

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download. For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu .

Format
Citation style
Style

To download the citation to this article, select your reference manager software.

There are no citations for this item

View options

Login options.

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR ® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Share article link

Copying failed.

NEXT ARTICLE

Request username.

Can't sign in? Forgot your username? Enter your email address below and we will send you your username

If the address matches an existing account you will receive an email with instructions to retrieve your username

Create a new account

Change password, password changed successfully.

Your password has been changed

Reset password

Can't sign in? Forgot your password?

Enter your email address below and we will send you the reset instructions

If the address matches an existing account you will receive an email with instructions to reset your password.

Your Phone has been verified

As described within the American Psychiatric Association (APA)'s Privacy Policy and Terms of Use , this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences. Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Published: 08 November 2021

Acute effects of alcohol on social and personal decision making

  • Hanna Karlsson 1   na1 ,
  • Emil Persson   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-2994-0541 2   na1 ,
  • Irene Perini   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-5972-0913 1 ,
  • Adam Yngve   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-1012-7286 1 ,
  • Markus Heilig 1   na1 &
  • Gustav Tinghög   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-8159-1249 2 , 3   na1  

Neuropsychopharmacology volume  47 ,  pages 824–831 ( 2022 ) Cite this article

15k Accesses

10 Citations

56 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Human behaviour
  • Risk factors

Social drinking is common, but it is unclear how moderate levels of alcohol influence decision making. Most prior studies have focused on adverse long-term effects on cognitive and executive function in people with alcohol use disorders (AUD). Some studies have investigated the acute effects of alcohol on decision making in healthy people, but have predominantly used small samples and focused on a narrow selection of tasks related to personal decision making, e.g., delay or probability discounting. Here, we conducted a large ( n  = 264), preregistered randomized placebo-controlled study (RCT) using a parallel group design, to systematically assess the acute effects of alcohol on measures of decision making in both personal and social domains. We found a robust effect of a 0.6 g/kg dose of alcohol on both moral judgment and altruistic behavior, but no effects on several measures of risk taking or waiting impulsivity. These findings suggest that alcohol at low to moderate doses selectively moderates decision making in the social domain, and promotes utilitarian decisions over those dictated by rule-based ethical principles (deontological). This is consistent with existing theory that emphasizes the dual roles of shortsighted information processing and salient social cues in shaping decisions made under the influence of alcohol. A better understanding of these effects is important to understand altered social functioning during alcohol intoxication.

Similar content being viewed by others

alcoholism research paper

Striatal activation to monetary reward is associated with alcohol reward sensitivity

alcoholism research paper

Impaired learning from regret and disappointment in alcohol use disorder

alcoholism research paper

Influences of social uncertainty and serotonin on gambling decisions

Introduction.

There is a lack of systematic research on the effects of moderate alcohol intake on decision making in non-clinical populations. This may be related to the difficulties that go into designing these types of studies, and the fact that prior research has been primarily focused on the adverse consequences of alcohol use disorders (AUD) on physiology and behavior. Numerous studies have investigated impairments in interpersonal behavior and decision-making processes in patients with AUD, but these studies cannot disaggregate the direct effects of alcohol from functional consequences of alcohol-induced organ damage, such as e.g., well documented alcohol-induced regional gray matter loss in AUD [ 1 ].

In healthy volunteers, alcohol intake can influence incentive motivation through activation of canonical dopaminergic brain reward system, but these effects vary by gender and genetics [ 2 , 3 , 4 , 5 ]. Enhanced emotional reactivity and increased positive mood have also been linked to alcohol intake in non-threatening environments [ 6 , 7 ]. It is furthermore widely held that alcohol results in broad and non-selective impairments of cognitive function, but this notion has recently been questioned. A meta-analysis of studies that examined the effects of alcohol on event-related potentials suggests that alcohol intake results in relatively selective impairments of attention, automatic auditory processing, and performance monitoring [ 8 ]. Similarly, alcohol is commonly held to increase impulsivity, but available studies make it difficult to disentangle to what extent impulsivity is a cause vs. a consequence of alcohol use, and also point to the moderating influence of emotional states [ 9 ].

Few studies have examined acute effects of alcohol on motivated behavior and decision making under a level of experimental control that allows causal inferences. For instance, many of the existing studies have used survey data to compare the behavior of people who abuse alcohol to those who do not. Although there are also placebo-controlled laboratory studies, most of these have used small samples and focused on a narrow selection of tasks related to personal decision making, primarily risk taking and impulsivity [ 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 , 14 , 15 , 16 , 17 , 18 , 19 , 20 ]. Even for these tasks, there is a lack of converging evidence. Some studies found increased risk taking due to alcohol [ 11 , 13 ], while others found no effect [ 10 , 12 , 14 , 15 , 19 , 20 ]. Similarly, waiting impulsivity has been found to increase [ 19 ] or decrease [ 16 ] following alcohol intake, but the majority of studies have found mixed or no effects [ 10 , 11 , 14 , 15 , 17 ]. Prototypical tasks for altruism and moral judgment have only been included in a minority of studies, with mixed results for both types of tasks [ 19 , 20 , 21 , 22 ]. In addition, some studies have used an observational field paradigm, typically approaching people in a bar with a structured questionnaire [ 22 , 23 , 24 ]. Whereas important insights can be obtained from these observational studies, they cannot provide answers about the causal relationship between alcohol intake and behavior, as they are inherently correlational, and also prone to selection bias.

Here, we therefore investigated how moderate acute alcohol intoxication influences basic social and personal decision making central to a wide variety of everyday behaviors: altruistic behavior and distributional preference, moral judgment, waiting impulsivity, and choice under risk. To this end, we conducted a preregistered (see https://osf.io/sf5em ) randomized placebo-controlled study, using a general task paradigm and a substantially larger sample ( n  = 264) than previous studies. We randomized participants to alcohol (0.6 and 0.51 g/kg for males and females, resp.) or placebo, and assessed moral judgment using standard sacrificial dilemmas (trolley problems) thought to probe the interaction between emotional intuitions and controlled cognitive processes in moral cognition [ 25 , 26 , 27 , 28 ]. Prosocial behavior was assessed using modified versions of the dictator game [ 29 , 30 ]. For risk taking, we used two different tasks, covering both intuitive-cognitive aspects of decision making, via standard prospect theory gambles [ 31 ], and more affect-laden decisions from experience, using the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART; [ 32 ]). Finally, waiting impulsivity was assessed using a prototypical task that captures participants’ preferences for real monetary rewards delivered at different points in time [ 33 , 34 ]. We assessed both general discounting (over relatively short delays) and temporal inconsistency in discounting, known as present bias, which is a characteristic property of discounting models that feature a sharp rise in the discounting rate for rewards delivered closer to today, such as quasi-hyperbolic discounting [ 34 , 35 ].

Materials and methods

Ethics statement.

The study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board of Linköping (ref 2016/496-31) and all participants provided written informed consent.

Open science

The preregistration together with data, analysis codes (main analyses), and experimental materials are available via the project’s OSF repository ( https://osf.io/sf5em ). Individual level data for the main analyses are shown in Supplementary materials Fig. S 1 –S 4 . We preregistered six main questions of interest for this data collection; this paper is focused on the first four of them.

Participants

Healthy volunteers were recruited using advertisements in social media, flyers, and the Online Recruitment System for Economic Experiments ORSEE [ 36 ] at Linköping University, Sweden. Eligible participants were randomized to alcohol ( n  = 128) or placebo ( n  = 136). The groups were similar in terms of baseline characteristics, including age, sex, education, alcohol consumption as measured with AUDIT, and personality traits measured with NEO-FFI (Table  1 ). The distribution of AUDIT scores was also very similar in both groups, and shown in Supplementary Materials (Fig. S 1 ). Our final sample size is smaller than the pre-specified target of n = 300 because we had to stop enrolling participants due to the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic.

Study timeline

The study visit consisted of five phases (Fig.  1B ): screening, questionnaires for baseline assessments, treatment phase (intake of drink), decision-making tasks performed at a computer, and a finishing phase with end of session questionnaires. The study was conducted in a computer lab in sessions of up to 15 participants, who were seated in separate cubicles and did not interact with each other.

figure 1

A CONSORT diagram of study participant. B study timeline. C time course of BrAC (mean ± SD).

Screening and eligibility

During the screening phase, prospective participants were evaluated for eligibility by a research nurse or a physician. Detailed eligibility criteria are provided in Supplementary Materials. In brief, subjects were excluded if they had any psychiatric disorder, were pregnant, had any previous neurological condition or if they were at risk of alcohol or other substance use disorders except nicotine. Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test [AUDIT; [ 37 ]] was used to assess the presence of AUD or hazardous drinking. Weight and sex were noted. Breath alcohol concentration (BrAC) baseline was measured using a breathalizer. A total of 316 individuals were evaluated, and 265 were included. Of these, 129 were allocated to placebo and 136 were assigned to alcohol (Fig.  1A ).

Baseline assessments

Baseline personality traits were obtained using the NEO Five Factor Inventory [NEO-FFI; [ 38 ]]. The Symptom checklist-90 [SCL-90; [ 39 ]] was used to measure symptoms of anxiety and depression. The Family Tree Questionnaire [FTQ; [ 40 ]] was used to assess family history of alcohol problems. The Biphasic Alcohol Effect Scale [BAES; [ 41 ]] was used to measure stimulant and sedative effects of alcohol.

Alcohol administration

Participants were informed that they would receive alcohol, corresponding to a BrAC of 0.6‰ or placebo, and were randomized to one of these in a parallel group design (see Fig.  1A ). In the alcohol group, male participants received a 0.6 g/kg dose of alcohol using a 12% solution. The solution was made using 95% ethanol mixed with cranberry juice. To adjust for known differences in body water, women received 85% of the alcohol administered to men. In the placebo group participants received a 1% alcohol solution. In both groups, the drink was divided into three glasses. Participants in both the alcohol and placebo group were required to finish each glass within five minutes. After the last glass, participants had a break for 15 min before proceeding with the decision-making tasks. Breath alcohol concentration (BrAC) was measured at baseline, 25 min later, just before the decision-making tasks and after additional appr. 45 min, as soon as the participant finished the session. The Biphasic Alcohol Effect Scale [BAES; [ 41 ]] was performed every time BrAC was measured and the Drug Effect Questionnaire [DEQ; [ 42 ]] was measured the second and third time BrAC was measured.

Decision-making tasks

For detailed task description and instructions, see Supplementary Materials. In brief, tasks focused on four domains of decision making: waiting impulsivity, choice under risk, moral judgment, and prosocial behavior. Tasks were presented on a computer screen using Qualtrics and Inquisit software. Divider screens prevented participants from seeing each other’s responses. Tasks were presented in a block-randomized order. At the end of the experiment, one decision for each subject was randomly selected and paid out for real (using the cell phone payment system Swish) together with the show-up fee of 150 SEK (appr. $15) that participants received for participating in the study.

Waiting impulsivity

This was assessed using a prototypical task that measures participants’ preferences for rewards delivered at different points in time [ 33 , 34 ]. Participants chose repeatedly between smaller rewards delivered sooner (SS) and larger rewards delivered later (LL). We tested for two distinct types of discounting; a general form of impatience, based on the proportion of smaller-sooner choices each person made in the first block of items ( pr. smaller-sooner ), and a specific form of impatience known as present bias, which is based on the difference (for each participant) between choices made in the first and second blocks of items ( diff. pr. smaller-sooner ). Present bias is a characteristic property of discounting models that feature a sharp rise in the discounting rate for rewards delivered closer to today, such as quasi-hyperbolic discounting [ 34 , 35 ].

Risk taking

One of the tasks to examine risk taking used standard prospect-theory gambles [ 31 ]. We used incentivized binary choices between a lottery and a certain amount of money in three different domains: gain, loss, mixed. We used the proportion of choices where the gamble was our main dependent variable for each domain ( pr. risky choices ). Using this task enabled us to characterize choices after the expected patterns of prospect theory [ 31 ], which emphasizes greater risk aversion for gains than losses and disproportionate weighting of the loss component in mixed prospects.

The second task in this domain was the Balloon Analog Risk Task [BART; [ 32 ]], in which participants were presented with a picture of a balloon and could earn money by pumping up the balloon by clicking a button. Each click earned them 0.1 SEK and caused the balloon to incrementally inflate. If the balloon was overinflated, it exploded, and all money earned for that trial was lost. If instead participants had chosen to cash-out prior to the balloon exploding, the money earned for that trial was added to their sum for this task. Our main dependent variable was the average number of pumps per trial, excluding trials where the balloon exploded ( avg. pumps per balloon ).

Moral judgment

This was assessed using four sacrificial moral dilemmas (trolley problems) that involved a conflict between utilitarian and deontological moral foundations [ 25 , 43 , 44 ]. In each dilemma, participants were faced with the possibility of saving a certain number of people by sacrificing one individual. Killing the single person while saving the others is consistent with utilitarian judgment, while not pulling the switch is consistent with deontological judgment, whereby actively causing harm to another person is morally unacceptable regardless of overall consequences. The main dependent variable for moral judgment was based on participants’ responses to four moral dilemmas (switch, footbridge, fumes, and shark; see Supplementary materials for details), presented in random order, and calculated as the proportion of utilitarian choices made by each participant ( pr. utilitarian choices ).

Prosocial behavior

This was assessed using two different tasks, designed to measure both altruistic behavior and preference for equality versus efficiency in distributions. Both were modified versions of the dictator game [ 29 , 30 ].

In the first task, participants were endowed with 50 SEK (appr. $5) and decided how much of it to keep for themselves and how much to donate to a well-known charity organization (Swedish Heart-Lung Foundation). The main dependent variable was the amount donated ( donation to charity ).

In the second task, subjects chose repeatedly between binary allocations of money (for themselves and another anonymous participant). Each item featured a choice between an equal distribution and an unequal but more efficient distribution, for example 40 SEK (appr. $4) each vs 40 SEK for me and 50 SEK for the other participant. We used the proportion (for each person) of choices where the equal allocation was chosen over the more efficient allocation ( pr. equality ).

Statistical analysis

The main analysis plan was specified before data collection begun, see the preregistration for details. STATISTICA 13.0 (Dell Inc, Tulsa, OK) was used for all analyses. One-way ANOVA, with group (alcohol or placebo) as a between-subject factor, and a pre-set alpha=0.05, were the preregistered main tests. Subject-level data for main tests are provided in Supplementary Materials, Fig. S 1 –S 4 . Secondary analyses (not preregistered) additionally assessed the potential influence of baseline subject characteristics (age, sex, personality measures, and alcohol use as measured by the AUDIT). Covariates were retained in analysis models if they were a significant predictor, or if they reduced the residual variance by more than 10%; otherwise, they were excluded. In additional analyses (also not preregistered) we compared self-reported effects of alcohol (stimulant, sedative, strength of drug effect, desirability) across the two conditions, based on subjects’ responses to the Biphasic Alcohol Effect Scale (BAES) and the Drug Effect Questionnaire (DEQ).

No BrAC alcohol was detected in the placebo group at any timepoint, or in the alcohol group at baseline. In the alcohol group, a BrAC of appr. 0.5‰ was reached by the time behavioral testing started, and remained stable at that level until completion of testing (Fig.  1C ). Using the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale [ 41 ], the alcohol group showed the expected stimulant as well as sedative effects of alcohol compared to the placebo group. On the Drug Effects Questionnaire [ 42 ], there was a clear effect of alcohol on the “Feel drug” and “High” items (Fig.  2 ). Neither “Like” nor “Want more” items were affected. The proportion of participants who correctly guessed their allocation was 95.5% in the alcohol group, and 69% in the placebo group. No unexpected adverse events were noted.

figure 2

A – D Mean responses on the Drug Effect Questionnaire (DEQ) before and after the decision-making tasks. Error bars indicate 95% Confidence Intervals. E , F Mean responses to the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale (BAES). Error bars indicate 95% Confidence Intervals. Significant alcohol effects for all items are indicated in the Results section.

Moral judgment in sacrificial dilemmas

Preference for utilitarian responding was increased in the alcohol group (one way ANOVA: F 1, 262  = 5.71, p  = 0.02; Cohen’s d = 0.29; Fig.  3A ). This remained unchanged when controlling for potential confounds. In the final ANCOVA, agreeableness ( p  < 0.01), gender ( p  = 0.06) and hazardous alcohol use, as measured with the AUDIT ([ 37 ]; p  = 0.02) contributed to the model, and all correlated negatively with utilitarian choices. Exploratory analyses indicated that the effect of alcohol on moral judgment was driven by the switch and fumes dilemmas, and to some extent the shark dilemma, while no corresponding effect was seen in the footbridge dilemma.

figure 3

A Moral judgment. Main panel: overall proportion of utilitarian choices. Inset: proportion of participants in each group who chose the utilitarian option, for the respective scenario. B Donation to charity. Main panel: Total amount of money donated. Inset: distribution of amounts donated to the charity, by group. Ten Swedish kronor (SEK) was approximately equal to one USD at the time of the experiment. Tick marks on the x-axis show the midpoints of equally-sized bins (10 SEK wide), except at the endpoints, where bin size is smaller. Error bars indicate 95% Confidence Intervals. Sample size is n  = 128 for placebo and n  = 136 for alcohol.

Participants in the alcohol group donated more money to a charity ( F 1, 262  = 4.83, p  = 0.03; Cohen’s d = 0.27; Fig.  3B ). This remained unchanged when controlling for potential confound of baseline subject characteristics. In the final model, agreeableness ( p  < 0.01) and hazardous alcohol use as measured with the AUDIT ( p  = 0.02) significantly contributed to the model. Agreeableness was positively correlated with donations and AUDIT was negatively correlated.

Equality/efficiency tradeoffs did not differ between groups (0.27 ± 0.38 vs. 0.27 ± 0.39; F 1, 262  < 0.01, p  = 0.98); thus, participants in both groups were reluctant to pursue equality of resources if redistribution had a cost. This result remained unchanged when controlling for potential confounds. In the final model, age ( p  < 0.01), neuroticism ( p  < 0.01), extraversion ( p  < 0.01), openness ( p  = 0.02), conscientiousness ( p  = 0.01) and gender ( p  < 0.01) significantly contributed to the model. Openness correlated negatively with equality. Female gender, age, neuroticism, extraversion and conscientiousness correlated positively with equality.

Risk taking – prospect theory gambles & BART

Behavior in the prospect gambles was similar in the two groups (Fig.  4 ). There was a tendency for decreased risk taking in the alcohol group for gains (0.59 ± 0.29 vs. 0.65 ± 0.22; F 1, 262  = 3.58, p  = 0.06), but no effect, or trend in the loss (0.49 ± 0.22 vs. 0.45 ± 0.22; F 1, 262  = 1.72, p  = 0.19), or in the mixed domain (0.49 ± 0.21 vs. 0.47 ± 0.22; F 1, 262  = 0.64, p  = 0.42). When all three domains were combined, the alcohol and placebo groups were virtually indistinguishable (0.52 ± 0.18 vs. 0.52 ± 0.15; F 1,262  < 0.01, p  = 0.96; Cohen’s d = −0.01). This remained unchanged when controlling for potential confounds. In the final model, age ( p  < 0.01), extraversion ( p  = 0.01), conscientiousness ( p  = 0.03) and agreeableness ( p  = 0.06) significantly contributed to the model or showed a tendency to do so. Age and extraversion were positively correlated with risk taking, while agreeableness and conscientiousness were negatively correlated with risk taking.

figure 4

A Mean proportion of trials where individuals chose the gamble over the certain option, separated by domain (gain, loss, mixed). Error bars indicate 95% Confidence Intervals calculated from t tests. B Distribution of the average number of pumps per balloon on the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART). Sample size is n  = 128 for placebo and n  = 136 for alcohol, except for BART where two individuals in placebo and three in alcohol could not participate in the task due to software issues.

Similarly, there was no difference in risk taking on the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART) between alcohol and placebo (Fig.  4 ; 43.4 ± 14.1 vs. 43.5 ± 14.2; F 1,257  < 0.01, p  = 0.99; Cohen’s d = −0.002). This remained unchanged when controlling for potential confounds. In the final model, neuroticism ( p  = 0.01) and conscientiousness ( p  = 0.05) were significant covariates. Both were negatively correlated with adjusted average number of pumps.

There was no statistically significant difference between groups for waiting impulsivity (0.24 ± 0.31 vs. 0.29 ± 0.31; F 1,262  = 2.21, p  = 0.14), or present bias (0.0007 ± 0.15 vs. 0.03 ± 0.18; F 1,262  = 2.59, p  = 0.11). Results were similar when all individual decisions were combined (0.24 ± 0.30 vs. 0.28 ± 0.29; F 1,262  = 1.25, p  = 0.26; Cohen’s d = −0.14). Thus, any possible effect of alcohol on waiting impulsivity was small and insignificant, and the bound on the 95% confidence interval in the hypothesized direction, i.e., increased waiting impulsivity following alcohol intake, was close to zero. These results remained unchanged when controlling for potential confounds.

We conducted a large, preregistered RCT to assess acute effects of alcohol on measures of decision making in personal and social domains. A 0.6 g/kg dose of alcohol did not influence personal decisions, but robustly moderated social decision making. In particular, subjects in the alcohol group showed an increased utilitarian preference in sacrificial moral dilemmas, and donated more money to charity in a modified dictator-game task. As an internal validation of these findings, we detected the expected effects of personality traits, independently of the alcohol effects. Although participants’ level of alcohol use, as measured by the AUDIT scale, correlated negatively both with their utilitarian decisions and charitable donations, the effects of alcohol on these outcomes did not interact with the level of alcohol use, and thus did not differ across the spectrum of use included in the study. For personal decision making, we did not find an effect of alcohol at the dose given on any of several risk-taking measures or waiting impulsivity. As an internal validation, we reliably replicated known patterns of results with all our tasks, e.g., increased risk seeking for losses and selective sensitivity to harmful actions across different moral dilemmas. Thus, our null findings are unlikely a result of compromised task calibration or unusual sample composition. Our findings are also unlikely to be explained by effects on elements of decision making that are related to impulse control, since, at the moderate level of alcohol intoxication used, we found no effects in tasks specifically designed to capture this dimension of behavior.

Our results for moral judgment, that subjects became increasingly utilitarian, differ from the few previous studies. Francis and colleagues [ 21 ] recently conducted a placebo-controlled study on moral judgment, using both traditional moral dilemmas and an adapted virtual-reality moral behavior task. They found no effects of alcohol on any of these tasks. In contrast, Duke and Bègue [ 22 ] found that alcohol intake correlated with increased utilitarian responding, but only on the footbridge dilemma and not on the switch dilemma, in a study conducted at two bars in France. However, the results from these two studies should be interpreted with caution, given the small sample sizes and the correlational nature of the data in the latter study. Our findings are contrary to what would be expected based on the widely held dual-process theory of moral cognition [ 25 , 28 ]. According to this theory, the effects of alcohol to increase emotional reactivity and weaken cognitive control should give increased preference for deontological rather than utilitarian actions. In fact, we find the opposite, i.e. increased utilitarian responding due to alcohol. A possible account of this finding is that acute alcohol intoxication primarily affects moral judgment through effects on its cognitive elements, and does so by subtly shifting the balance between perceived costs and benefits in the utilitarian calculation. This is broadly consistent with findings indicating an important role of frontocortical brain areas in social decision making [ 45 ], and a higher sensitivity of these neocortical structures to alcohol effects compared to subcortical brain structures that generate incentive salience and affective signals [ 1 ].

Acute effects of alcohol on altruistic behavior using real monetary rewards have hardly been assessed at all previously. Two previous studies found no effect or a tendency for a negative effect on altruism following alcohol intake [ 19 , 20 ]. In contrast, we found that alcohol made people more altruistic, donating a larger proportion of their money (around ten percentage points more than the placebo group) to charity. This is a modest effect size, but appears to be highly specific, as it was found at a modest dose of alcohol at which there were no discernible effects on impulsivity or risk taking. We had no a priori expectation about the direction of the effect on altruism. In principle, these results can also be rationalized using alcohol myopia theory [ 46 , 47 , 48 ], which emphasizes impaired attention and thus increased reliance on salient stimuli following acute alcohol intoxication. The need of the charity recipients is arguably a salient cue in the task that we used, and it is possible that this is what caused increased donations in the alcohol group.

Previous studies on personal decision making for risk and impulsivity have found mixed results [ 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 , 14 , 15 , 16 , 17 , 18 , 19 , 20 , 49 ], but most studies have been limited by a small sample size. Prior to our study, Bernhardt et al. [ 10 ] was probably the most well-powered study to date ( n  = 54 adolescent males in a within-subject design), and their results are similar to what we found, with no effects on waiting impulsivity or on risk taking in gain, loss, or mixed domains. Taken together, this strongly suggests that alcohol taken at moderated doses by healthy social drinkers has small or no effects on risk taking or waiting impulsivity. For the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART), we are aware of only one previous study that was adequately powered, Rose et al. [ 50 ] with n  = 142 in a between-subjects design; e.g., all other studies reviewed by Harmon et al. [ 51 ] had <33 subjects per treatment cell. Interestingly, whereas Rose et al. found increased risk taking (more pumps) due to alcohol intake (Cohen’s d = 0.40 at a 0.6 g/kg dose of alcohol), our results clearly favored a no-effects interpretation, with the 95% confidence interval bounded at an effect size or appr. Cohen’s d = 0.25 in either direction. Thus, more studies are needed to determine the acute effects alcohol on the BART. Of note, while the BART is commonly viewed as a generic “risk taking task”, its original evaluation suggested that it may in fact be more related to sensation seeking and impaired behavioral inhibition [ 32 ], i.e. facets of the impulsivity distinct from those involved in trading off the magnitude of gains or losses vs. their probability.

Our study has several strengths as well as limitations. Among the former, it had a large sample size and a preregistered analysis plan. This is important given that prior studies are for the most part small and without transparent control of analytical flexibility. The combination of small sample sizes, high analytical flexibility and publication bias has been a perfect storm for generating irreproducible findings [ 52 , 53 , 54 , 55 ]. However, despite a larger sample than previous studies, we had insufficient power to conduct otherwise relevant subgroup analyses, for example based on gender or quantitative traits, beyond using them as covariates in the analysis. For the same reason, we did not attempt to capture biphasic effects of alcohol. Finally, we were not able to control for expectation effects by adding more conditions, while blinding was not successful. These limitations may affect the generalizability of our findings.

Some features of the study are both strengths and limitations. For instance, we ensured a high degree of experimental control, at the expense of assessing the effects of alcohol in a standardized, sterile laboratory environment. As expected under these conditions, while self-ratings of intoxication (“feeling effect” and “high”) were robustly influenced by alcohol, neither “liking” nor “wanting” ratings were affected. On one hand, this suggests that our findings are unlikely to be primarily driven by expectations, since expectations of alcohol effects are linked to experiencing alcohol in a naturalistic context. At the same time, alcohol effects on decision making under laboratory conditions may differ from those “in the wild”. Similarly, although we make a distinction between personal and social decision making in terms of outcomes, all decisions in our study were taken in private in front of a computer. Thus, future studies could extend our findings by investigating the effects of alcohol on social decisions made in a public setting (e.g., observed by an audience), where social signaling and reputational concerns also come into play.

Designing the experiment, we emphasized task comprehension, and all decisions that involved money were incentivized (participants were paid for one randomly drawn decision at the end). Payments were implemented via a standard cell phone transfer system in order to circumvent concerns about differential transactions costs in the waiting-impulsivity task [ 56 ]. However, as a potential side effect, this made the larger-later option in this task more attractive than we had anticipated, resulting in a more than usual amount of upper censoring (people who chose the larger-later option for all trials) for this task. Our results for waiting impulsivity should be interpreted with this limitation in mind. Similarly, our finding that alcohol did not influence impulsivity, may not generalize to higher doses, or other populations. Also, even at the dose used, effects on impulsivity might be present in people with substance use disorders, externalizing psychopathology, or both.

The pattern of our results suggests that alcohol selectively moderates decision making in the social domain, at least for low-moderate doses of alcohol. This is consistent with existing theory that emphasizes the dual roles of shortsighted information processing and salient social cues in shaping decisions under the influence of alcohol [ 46 ]. Our findings are obtained in social drinkers without any AUD, but have potentially important implications for attempts to understand the emergence of AUD. Most prior alcohol challenge studies have focused exclusively on personal decision making, but changes in social cognition, ultimately resulting in social marginalization and exclusion, are at the core of the addictive process [ 57 , 58 ]. It has recently been shown that communicating deontologically rather than utilitarian-motivated decisions may be more advantageous to signal trustworthiness as group member [ 59 , 60 ]. Impairments in the ability to signal trustworthiness caused by alcohol use could contribute to social marginalization. These alcohol-induced effects on social cognition are likely to interact with pre-existing vulnerabilities to influence social functioning. Our findings highlight the importance of taking the social dimension of decision making into account to better understand the process of developing AUD.

Taking a broader perspective, to policymakers and everyday decision-makers alike, it is useful to know that the influence of alcohol on decision making is sensitive to social cues. Whether alcohol is ultimately good or bad for people’s decisions will likely depend on context. Perhaps surprisingly, from the narrow perspective of our sample and the specific tasks that we used, social outcomes were more advantageous among people who were given alcohol compared to people who were not.

Xiao P, Dai Z, Zhong J, Zhu Y, Shi H, Pan P. Regional gray matter deficits in alcohol dependence: a meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;153:22–28.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Boileau I, Assaad JM, Pihl RO, Benkelfat C, Leyton M, Diksic M, et al. Alcohol promotes dopamine release in the human nucleus accumbens. Synapse. 2003;49:226–31.

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Gilman JM, Ramchandani VA, Davis MB, Bjork JM, Hommer DW. Why we like to drink: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the rewarding and anxiolytic effects of alcohol. J Neurosci 2008;28:4583–91.

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Urban NB, Kegeles LS, Slifstein M, Xu X, Martinez D, Sakr E, et al. Sex differences in striatal dopamine release in young adults after oral alcohol challenge: a positron emission tomography imaging study with [(11)C]raclopride. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:689–96.

Ramchandani VA, Umhau J, Pavon FJ, Ruiz-Velasco V, Margas W, Sun H, et al. A genetic determinant of the striatal dopamine response to alcohol in men. Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16:809–17.

Fairbairn CE, Sayette MA. The effect of alcohol on emotional inertia: a test of alcohol myopia. J Abnorm Psychol 2013;122:770–81.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Sayette MA, Creswell KG, Dimoff JD, Fairbairn CE, Cohn JF, Heckman BW, et al. Alcohol and Group Formation: A Multimodal Investigation of the Effects of Alcohol on Emotion and Social Bonding. Psychol Sci 2012;23:869–78.

CE Fairbairn, D Kang, KD Federmeier, Alcohol and neural dynamics: a meta-analysis of acute alcohol effects on event-related brain potentials. Biol Psychiatry https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.11.024 (2020).

Herman AM, Duka T. Facets of impulsivity and alcohol use: what role do emotions play? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2019;106:202–16.

Bernhardt N, Obst E, Nebe S, Pooseh S, Wurst FM, Weinmann W, et al. Acute alcohol effects on impulsive choice in adolescents. J Psychopharmacol. 2019;33:316–25.

Bidwell LC, MacKillop J, Murphy JG, Grenga A, Swift RM, McGeary JE. Biphasic effects of alcohol on delay and probability discounting. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2013;21:214–21.

Balodis IM, MacDonald TK, Olmstead MC. Instructional cues modify performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain Cogn. 2006;60:109–17.

Lane SD, Cherek DR, Pietras CJ, Tcheremissine OV. Alcohol effects on human risk taking. Psychopharmacol. 2004;172:68–77.

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Richards JB, Zhang L, Mitchell SH, de Wit H. Delay or probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: effect of alcohol. J Exp Anal Behav. 1999;71:121–43.

Reynolds B, Richards JB, de Wit H. Acute-alcohol effects on the Experiential Discounting Task (EDT) and a question-based measure of delay discounting. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2006;83:194–202.

Ortner CN, MacDonald TK, Olmstead MC. Alcohol intoxication reduces impulsivity in the delay-discounting paradigm. Alcohol Alcohol. 2003;38:151–6.

Adams S, Attwood AS, Munafò MR. Drinking status but not acute alcohol consumption influences delay discounting. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2017;32:e2617.

Article   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

George S, Rogers RD, Duka T. The acute effect of alcohol on decision making in social drinkers. Psychopharmacol. 2005;182:160–9.

Corazzini L, Filippin A, Vanin P. Economic behavior under the influence of alcohol: an experiment on time preferences, risk-taking, and altruism. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0121530.

Bregu K, Deck C, Ham L, Jahedi S. The effects of alcohol use on economic decision making. South Economic J. 2017;83:886–902.

Article   Google Scholar  

Francis KB, Gummerum M, Ganis G, Howard IS, Terbeck S. Alcohol, empathy, and morality: acute effects of alcohol consumption on affective empathy and moral decision-making. Psychopharmacol. 2019;236:3477–96.

Duke AA, Bègue L. The drunk utilitarian: blood alcohol concentration predicts utilitarian responses in moral dilemmas. Cognition. 2015;134:121–7.

Burghart DR, Glimcher PW, Lazzaro SC. An expected utility maximizer walks into a bar…. J risk Uncertain. 2013;46:215–46.

Proestakis A, Espín AM, Exadaktylos F, Cortés Aguilar A, Oyediran OA, Palacio LA. The separate effects of self-estimated and actual alcohol intoxication on risk taking: a field experiment. J Neurosci Psychol Econ. 2013;6:115–35.

Greene JD, Nystrom LE, Engell AD, Darley JM, Cohen JD. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron. 2004;44:389–400.

Tinghög G, Andersson D, Bonn C, Johannesson M, Kirchler M, Koppel L, et al. Intuition and moral decision-making – the effect of time pressure and cognitive load on moral judgment and altruistic behavior. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0164012.

Persson E, Heilig M, Tinghög G, Capusan AJ. Using quantitative trait in adults with ADHD to test predictions of dual-process theory. Sci Rep. 2020;10:20076.

JD Greene, L Young, “The cognitive neuroscience of moral judgment and decision-making” in The Cognitive Neurosciences, D Poeppel, GR Mangun, MS Gazzaniga, Eds. (MIT Press, 2020).

Forsythe R, Horowitz JL, Savin NE, Sefton M. Fairness in simple bargaining experiments. Games Economic Behav. 1994;6:347–69.

Kahneman D, Knetsch JL, Thaler R. Fairness as a constraint on profit seeking: entitlements in the market. Am Economic Rev. 1986;76:728–41.

Google Scholar  

Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 1979;47:263–91.

Lejuez CW, Read JP, Kahler CW, Richards JB, Ramsey SE, Stuart GL, et al. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). J Exp Psychol Appl. 2002;8:75–84.

Hariri AR, Brown SM, Williamson DE, Flory JD, de Wit H, Manuck SB. Preference for immediate over delayed rewards is associated with magnitude of ventral striatal activity. J Neurosci. 2006;26:13213–7.

McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science. 2004;306:503–7.

Laibson D. Golden eggs and hyperbolic discounting. Q J Econ. 1997;112:443–78.

Greiner B. Subject pool recruitment procedures: organizing experiments with ORSEE. J Economic Sci Assoc. 2015;1:114–25.

Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption-II. Addiction. 1993;88:791–804.

PT Costa, RR McCrae, Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEP five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI): professional manual (Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa, Fla. (P.O. Box 998, Odessa 33556), 1992), pp. vi, 101 p.

L Derogatis, SCL-90-R: Manual-II (Clinical Psychmetric Research, Towson, MD, 1983).

Mann RE, Sobell LC, Sobell MB, Pavan D. Reliability of a family tree questionnaire for assessing family history of alcohol problems. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1985;15:61–67.

Martin CS, Earleywine M, Musty RE, Perrine MW, Swift RM. Development and validation of the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1993;17:140–6.

Morean ME, de Wit H, King AC, Sofuoglu M, Rueger SY, O'Malley SS. The drug effects questionnaire: psychometric support across three drug types. Psychopharmacol. 2013;227:177–92.

Thomson JJ. The trolley problem. Yale Law J. 1985;94:1395–415.

P Foot, Virtues and Vices and Other Essays in Moral Philosophy (Clarendon, Oxford. First published in 1978 by Blackwell publisher and University of California press, 2002).

Knoch D, Pascual-Leone A, Meyer K, Treyer V, Fehr E. Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science. 2006;314:829–32.

Steele CM, Josephs RA. Alcohol myopia. Its prized and dangerous effects. Am Psychol. 1990;45:921–33.

Steele CM, Southwick L. Alcohol and social behavior I: The psychology of drunken excess. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1985;48:18–34.

Steele CM, Critchlow B, Liu TJ. Alcohol and social behavior II: the helpful drunkard. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1985;48:35–46.

Breslin FC, Sobell MB, Cappell H, Vakili S, Poulos CX. The effects of alcohol, gender, and sensation seeking on the gambling choices of social drinkers. Psychol Addict Behav. 1999;13:243–52.

Rose AK, Jones A, Clarke N, Christiansen P. Alcohol-induced risk taking on the BART mediates alcohol priming. Psychopharmacol. 2014;231:2273–80.

Harmon DA, Haas AL, Peterkin A. Experimental tasks of behavioral risk taking in alcohol administration studies: a systematic review. Addict Behav. 2021;113:106678.

Open Science Collaboration, Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349 (2015).

Munafò MR, Nosek BA, Bishop D, Button KS, Chambers CD, du Sert NP, et al. A manifesto for reproducible science. Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1:0021.

Ioannidis JPA. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2005;2:e124.

Begley CG, Ellis LM. Raise standards for preclinical cancer research. Nature. 2012;483:531–3.

Cohen JD, Ericson KM, Laibson D, White JM. Measuring time preferences. J Econ Lit. 2020;58:299–347.

Heilig M, Epstein DH, Nader MA, Shaham Y. Time to connect: bringing social context into addiction neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17:592–9.

Heilig M, MacKillop J, Martinez D, Rehm J, Leggio L, Vanderschuren LJMJ. Addiction as a brain disease revised: why it still matters, and the need for consilience. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-00950-y .

Sacco DF, Brown M, Lustgraaf CJN, Hugenberg K. The adaptive utility of deontology: deontological moral decision-making fosters perceptions of trust and likeability. Evolut Psychological Sci. 2017;3:125–32.

Everett JA, Pizarro DA, Crockett MJ. Inference of trustworthiness from intuitive moral judgments. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2016;145:772–87.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Åsa Axén, Sandra Boda, Sarah Gustavson, Lisbet Severin, Lina Koppel, Theodor Arlestig and David Andersson for assisting with data collection.

This work was supported by the Swedish Research Council (MH: 2013-07434; GT: 2018-01755) and the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare (EP: 2020-00864). Funders had no role in study design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

These authors contributed equally: Hanna Karlsson, Emil Persson, Markus Heilig, Gustav Tinghög.

Authors and Affiliations

Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Department of Biomedical and Clinical Sciences, Linköping University, 581 83, Linköping, Sweden

Hanna Karlsson, Irene Perini, Adam Yngve & Markus Heilig

Department of Management and Engineering, Division of Economics, Linköping University, 581 83, Linköping, Sweden

Emil Persson & Gustav Tinghög

The National Center for Priority Setting in Health Care, Department of Medical and Health Sciences, Linköping University, 581 83, Linköping, Sweden

Gustav Tinghög

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

MH and GT provided funding for the study. HK, EP, IP, MH, and GT designed the study. HK, AY and GT collected the data. HK and EP analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. All authors revised the manuscript and approved the final manuscript for submission.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Markus Heilig .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

MH has received consulting fees, research support or other compensation from Indvior, Camurus, BrainsWay, Aelis Farma, and Janssen Pharmaceuticals. All other authors declare no conflicting interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary materials, rights and permissions.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Karlsson, H., Persson, E., Perini, I. et al. Acute effects of alcohol on social and personal decision making. Neuropsychopharmacol. 47 , 824–831 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01218-9

Download citation

Received : 18 June 2021

Revised : 12 October 2021

Accepted : 16 October 2021

Published : 08 November 2021

Issue Date : March 2022

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-021-01218-9

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

Neurofilament light chain and glial fibrillary acid protein levels are elevated in post-mild covid-19 or asymptomatic sars-cov-2 cases.

  • Domenico Plantone
  • Angela Stufano
  • Nicola De Stefano

Scientific Reports (2024)

Modeling social cognition in alcohol use disorder: lessons from schizophrenia

  • Irene Perini
  • Arthur Pabst
  • Markus Heilig

Psychopharmacology (2024)

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

alcoholism research paper

alcoholism research paper

An official website of the United States government

Here’s how you know

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you’ve safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

Home

The NIAAA is the lead agency for U.S. research on the causes, consequences, prevention and treatment of alcohol use disorder and alcohol-related problems.

National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA)

Major research initiatives.

Addressing alcohol-related issues—from basic science to clinical studies.

Extramural Research: Research at Grantee Institutions

Primary areas of research, funding opportunities, and staff listings for Extramural Research Divisions.

Intramural Research: Research in NIAAA Labs

Organization, primary areas of research, and staff listings for Intramural Research Labs.

A woman writing on a paper

Guidance and policies for scientists pursuing alcohol research.

decorative image of NIAAA strategic plan cover

The Strategic Plan describes research goals, themes, and programs.

logo of the alcohol research current reviews journal

NIAAA’s peer-reviewed scientific journal.

SBIR Research Thumbnail Image

About NIAAA's Small Business Research Funding Programs

Medical blocks

NIAAA surveillance reports on alcohol-related problems.

Research on factors that compel youth to begin and continue drinking.

Research on medications in development for treatment of alcohol use disorder.

The NIAAA data archive is a data repository that houses and shares human subjects data generated by NIAAA-funded research. 

Resources include biological specimens, animals, data, materials, tools, or services made available to  any qualified investigato r to accelerate alcohol-related research in a cost-effective manner.

Current and potential alcohol research investigators and trainees are encouraged to subscribe to our new email list to receive NIAAA information and updates relevant to the research community. To sign up, enter your contact information below.

Sign Up for Email Updates

niaaa.nih.gov

An official website of the National Institutes of Health and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Alcohol Res Health
  • v.24(1); 2000

Logo of arh

Health Risks and Benefits of Alcohol Consumption

Alcohol consumption has consequences for the health and well-being of those who drink and, by extension, the lives of those around them. The research reviewed here represents a wide spectrum of approaches to understanding the risks and benefits of alcohol consumption. These research findings can help shape the efforts of communities to reduce the negative consequences of alcohol consumption, assist health practitioners in advising consumers, and help individuals make informed decisions about drinking.

Forty-four percent of the adult U.S. population (age 18 and over) are current drinkers who have consumed at least 12 drinks in the preceding year ( Dawson et al. 1995 ). Although most people who drink do so safely, the minority who consume alcohol heavily produce an impact that ripples outward to encompass their families, friends, and communities. The following statistics give a glimpse of the magnitude of problem drinking:

  • Approximately 14 million Americans—7.4 percent of the population—meet the diagnostic criteria for alcohol abuse or alcoholism ( Grant et al. 1994 ).
  • More than one-half of American adults have a close family member who has or has had alcoholism ( Dawson and Grant 1998 ).
  • Approximately one in four children younger than 18 years old in the United States is exposed to alcohol abuse or alcohol dependence in the family ( Grant 2000 ).

Measuring the Health Risks and Benefits of Alcohol

Over the years, scientists have documented the effects of alcohol on many of the body’s organ systems and its role in the development of a variety of medical problems, including cardiovascular diseases, liver cirrhosis, and fetal abnormalities. Alcohol use and abuse also contribute to injuries, automobile collisions, and violence. Alcohol can markedly affect worker productivity and absenteeism, family interactions, and school performance, and it can kill, directly or indirectly. On the strength of this evidence, the United States and other countries have expended considerable effort throughout this century to develop and refine effective strategies to limit the negative impact of alcohol ( Bruun et al. 1975 ; Edwards et al. 1994 ).

In the past two decades, however, a growing number of epidemiologic studies have documented an association between alcohol consumption and lower risk for coronary heart disease (CHD), the leading cause of death in many developed countries ( Chadwick and Goode 1998 ; Criqui 1996 a , b ; Zakhari 1997 ). Much remains to be learned about this association, the extent to which it is due specifically to alcohol and not to other associated lifestyle factors, and what the biological mechanisms of such an effect might be.

Effects on Physical Health

Cardiovascular diseases account for more deaths among Americans than any other group of diseases. Several large prospective studies have reported a reduced risk of death from CHD across a wide range of alcohol consumption levels. These include studies among men in the United Kingdom ( Doll et al. 1994 ), Germany ( Keil et al. 1997 ), Japan ( Kitamura et al. 1998 ), and more than 85,000 U.S. women enrolled in the Nurses’ Health Study ( Fuchs et al. 1995 ). In research studies, definitions of moderate drinking vary. However, in these studies, most, if not all, of the apparent protective effect against CHD was realized at low to moderate levels of alcohol consumption.

Follow-up of another large U.S. survey, the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey I ( Rehm et al. 1997 ), found that after an average of nearly 15 years of follow-up, the incidence of CHD in men who drank was lower across all levels of consumption than in nondrinkers. Incidence also was reduced among women, but only in those consuming low to moderate levels of alcohol. In fact, an increased risk was observed in women consuming more than 28 drinks per week.

An association between moderate drinking and lower risk for CHD does not necessarily mean that alcohol itself is the cause of the lower risk. For example, a review of population studies indicates that the higher mortality risk among abstainers may be attributable to socioeconomic and employment status, mental health, overall health, and health habits such as smoking, rather than participants’ nonuse of alcohol ( Fillmore 1998 ).

It is also important to note that the apparent benefits of moderate drinking on CHD mortality are offset at higher drinking levels by increased risk of death from other types of heart disease, cancer, liver cirrhosis, and trauma. The U.S. Department of Agriculture (USDA) and the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (USDHHS), in the U.S. Dietary Guidelines for Americans, have defined moderate drinking as one drink per day or less for women and two or fewer drinks per day for men ( USDA 1995 ). In addition, the NIAAA further recommends that people aged 65 and older limit their consumption of alcohol to one drink per day.

Cerebrovascular disease, in which arteries in the brain are blocked or narrowed, can lead to a sudden, severe disruption of blood supply to the brain, called a stroke. Ischemic stroke, which is by far the predominant type of stroke, results from a blockage of a blood vessel; hemorrhagic stroke is due to rupture of a blood vessel. Alcohol-related hypertension, or high blood pressure, may increase the risk of both forms of stroke. Yet, in people with normal blood pressure, the risk of ischemic stroke may be decreased due to the apparent ability of alcohol to lessen damage to blood vessels due to lipid deposits and to reduce blood clotting. Alcohol’s anticlotting effects, while perhaps decreasing the risk of ischemic stroke, may increase the risk of hemorrhagic stroke ( Hillbom and Juvela 1996 ). These studies are coming closer to providing a clear picture of the relationship between alcohol and risk of stroke.

The relationship between alcohol consumption and stroke risk has been examined in two recent overviews. In a meta-analysis, researchers compared the relationship between alcohol consumption and the risk of ischemic and hemorrhagic strokes ( English et al. 1995 ). They detected no differences in the risk patterns for the two types of stroke, but found clear evidence that heavy drinking was associated with increased stroke risk, particularly in women.

In contrast, the Cancer Prevention Study II found that, in men, all levels of drinking were associated with a significant decrease in the risk of stroke death, but in women, the decreased risk was significant only among those consuming one drink or less daily ( Thun et al. 1997 ). A recent study reported that among male physicians in the Physicians’ Health Study, those who consumed more than one drink a week had a reduced overall risk of stroke compared with participants who had less than one drink per week ( Berger et al. 1999 ).

Among young people, long-term heavy alcohol consumption has been identified as an important risk factor for stroke ( You et al. 1997 ). Very recent alcohol drinking, particularly drinking to intoxication, has been found to be associated with a significant increase in the risk of ischemic stroke in both men and women aged 16 through 40 years ( Hillbom et al. 1995 ).

The relationship between alcohol consumption and blood pressure is noteworthy because hypertension is a major risk factor for stroke as well as for CHD. A national consensus panel in Canada recently conducted an extensive review of the evidence concerning this relationship ( Campbell et al. 1999 ), concluding that studies have consistently observed an association between heavy alcohol consumption and increased blood pressure in both men and women. However, in many studies comparing lower levels of alcohol use with abstention, findings are mixed. Some studies have found low alcohol consumption to have no effect on blood pressure or to result in a small reduction, while in other studies blood pressure levels increased as alcohol consumption increased.

The possibility that alcohol may protect against CHD has led researchers to hypothesize that alcohol may protect against peripheral vascular disease, a condition in which blood flow to the extremities is impaired due to narrowing of the blood vessels. In a 1985 analysis of data from the Framingham Heart Study, alcohol was not found to have a significant relationship, either harmful or protective, with peripheral vascular disease ( Kannel and McGee 1985 ). However, an important recent study produced different results. In an analysis of the 11-year follow-up data from more than 22,000 men enrolled in the Physicians’ Health Study, researchers found that daily drinkers who consumed seven or more drinks per week had a 26-percent reduction in risk of peripheral vascular disease ( Camargo et al. 1997 ).

Two other studies found inconsistent results with regard to gender. One study of middle-aged and older men and women in Scotland showed that as alcohol consumption increased, the prevalence of peripheral vascular disease declined in men but not in women ( Jepson et al. 1995 ). In contrast, among people with non-insulin-dependent diabetes, alcohol was associated with a lower prevalence of peripheral vascular disease in women but not in men ( Mingardi et al. 1997 ).

There is no question that alcohol abuse contributes significantly to liver-related morbidity (illness) and mortality in the United States. The effects of alcohol on the liver include inflammation (alcoholic hepatitis) and cirrhosis (progressive liver scarring). The risk for liver disease is related to how much a person drinks: the risk is low at low levels of alcohol consumption but increases steeply with higher levels of consumption ( Edwards et al. 1994 ). Gender also may play a role in the development of alcohol-induced liver damage. Some evidence indicates that women are more susceptible than men to the cumulative effects of alcohol on the liver ( Becker et al. 1996 ; Gavaler and Arria 1995 ; Hisatomi et al. 1997 ; Naveau et al. 1997 ).

Definitions Related to Drinking

Studies investigating the health effects of alcohol vary in their definitions of “low,” “moderate,” and “heavy” drinking. According to the Dietary Guidelines for Americans , issued jointly by the U.S. Department of Agriculture (USDA) and the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (USDHHS), moderate drinking is no more than two standard drinks per day for men and no more than one per day for women ( USDA and USDHHS 1995 ). The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism further recommends that people aged 65 and older limit their consumption of alcohol to one drink per day. Information on drinking levels as they are defined in the individual studies cited in this issue can be found in the original references.

How Much Is a Drink?

In the United States, a drink is considered to be 0.5 ounces (oz) or 15 grams of alcohol, which is equivalent to 12 oz (355 milliliters [mL]) of beer, 5 oz (148 mL) of wine, or 1.5 oz (44 mL) of 80-proof distilled spirits.

Does Abstaining Increase Risk?

Epidemiologic evidence has shown that people who drink alcohol heavily are at increased risk for a number of health problems. But some studies described in this section suggest that individuals who abstain from using alcohol also may be at greater risk for a variety of conditions or outcomes, particularly coronary heart disease, than persons who consume small to moderate amounts of alcohol.

This type of relationship may be expressed as a J-shaped or U-shaped curve, which means that the risk of a disease outcome from low to moderate drinking is less than the risk for either abstinence or heavier drinking, producing a curve in the shape of the letter J or U (see figure ).

By examining the lifestyle characteristics of people who consume either no alcohol or varying amounts of alcohol, researchers may uncover other factors that might account for different health outcomes. For example, gender, age, education, physical fitness, diet, and social involvement are among the factors that may be taken into account in determining relative risk of disease.

Similarly, people may quit drinking because of health problems, or even if that is not the case, former drinkers may have characteristics that contribute to their higher mortality risk, such as smoking, drug use, and lower socioeconomic status. If former drinkers are included in the abstainers group, they may make alcohol appear to be more beneficial than it is. Therefore the best research studies will distinguish between former drinkers and those who have never used alcohol.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is arcr-24-1-5f1.jpg

Rates of death from all causes, all cardiovascular diseases, and alcohol-augmented conditions from 1982 to 1991, according to base-line alcohol consumption.

SOURCE: Thun et al. 1997 . Reprinted with permission from New England Journal of Medicine , Vol. 337, pp. 1705–1714, 1997. Copyright 1997, Massachusetts Medical Society. Waltham, MA. All rights reserved.

  • U.S. Department of Agriculture and U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Home and Garden Bulletin No. 232. 4th ed. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Agriculture; 1995. [ Google Scholar ]

Alcohol has been linked to a number of cancers, including cancers of the head and neck (mouth, pharynx, larynx, and esophagus), digestive tract (stomach, colon, and rectum) and breast ( World Cancer Research Fund/American Institute for Cancer Research [WCRF/AICR] 1997 ; Doll et al. 1993 ; International Agency for Research on Cancer [IARC] 1988 ).

Alcohol is clearly established as a cause of cancer of various tissues in the airway and digestive tract, including the mouth, pharynx, larynx, and esophagus ( Doll et al. 1993 ; IARC 1988 ; La Vecchia and Negri 1989 ; Seitz and Pöschl 1997 ; WCRF/AICR 1997 ). An increased risk of gastric or stomach cancer among alcohol drinkers has been identified in several, but not the majority, of case-control or cohort studies. The link between alcohol use and chronic gastritis (stomach inflammation) is clear, although progression from chronic gastritis to neoplasia is less well understood and probably involves other factors in addition to alcohol ( Bode and Bode 1992 , 1997 ).

In addition, a link between alcohol and breast cancer has been suspected for two decades but the nature of this association remains unclear. (For a more detailed discussion of the role of alcohol in breast cancer, see the article in this issue on medical consequences pp 27–31.)

Psychosocial Consequences and Cognitive Effects

Alcohol use plays a role in many social activities, from the “business lunch” and parties to special occasions. The benefits to those who drink during social occasions are greatly influenced by culture, the setting in which drinking occurs, and expectations about alcohol’s effects ( Goldman et al. 1987 ; Heath 1987 ; Leigh 1989 ; Leigh and Stacy 1991 ). Stress reduction, mood elevation, increased sociability, and relaxation are the most commonly reported psychosocial benefits of drinking alcohol ( Baum-Baicker 1985 ; Hauge and Irgens-Jensen 1990 ; Leigh and Stacy 1991 ; Mäkelä and Mustonen 1988 ).

There is extensive evidence indicating that people who suffer psychological distress and rely on alcohol to relieve their stress are more likely to develop alcohol abuse and dependence ( Castaneda and Cushman 1989 ; Kessler et al. 1996 , 1997 ). Because vulnerability to alcohol dependence varies greatly among individuals, it is difficult to assess the risk of dependence in relation to how much a person drinks. Two persons exposed to alcohol in exactly the same way may or may not have the same outcome for many reasons, including genetic differences, personality, behavioral features, and environment.

Most mental disorders occur much more often than expected by chance among people who are abusing alcohol or are alcohol dependent ( Kessler et al. 1996 ). Of these individuals, those who are alcohol dependent are more likely than alcohol abusers to have mental disorders. In fact, alcohol dependence elevates the risk for all types of affective and anxiety disorders ( Kessler et al. 1996 ).

Although the relationship between heavy alcohol consumption and cognitive impairment is well established, the effects of moderate drinking on the ability to perform cognitive tasks, including remembering, reasoning, and thinking, are largely unexplored.

Most studies of the relationship between alcohol consumption and other forms of dementia, notably Alzheimer’s disease ( Tyas 1996 ), have failed to find statistically significant associations. However, several recent studies suggest that moderate alcohol consumption may have a positive effect on cognitive function. In an analysis of baseline data (data collected at the beginning of a study) for persons aged 59 through 71 who were enrolled in the Epidemiology of Vascular Aging Study in France, moderate alcohol consumption was associated with higher cognitive functioning among women but not men after a number of possible confounding variables were controlled for ( Dufouil et al. 1997 ). Another study, which followed 3,777 community residents in France who drank primarily wine, found a markedly reduced risk of the incidence of dementia among moderate drinkers relative to abstainers ( Orgogozo et al. 1997 ).

Effects on Society

Researchers have identified and classified a wide variety of adverse consequences for people who drink and their families, friends, co-workers, and others they encounter ( Edwards et al. 1994 ; Harford et al. 1991 ; Hilton 1991 a , b ). Alcohol-related problems include economic losses resulting from time off work owing to alcohol-related illness and injury, disruption of family and social relationships, emotional problems, impact on perceived health, violence and aggression, and legal problems.

The risk of such consequences for the individual varies widely and depends on the situation. However, researchers have found a general trend toward an increased risk of adverse effects on society as the average alcohol intake among individuals increases ( Mäkelä and Mustonen 1988 ; Mäkelä and Simpura 1985 ).

Alcohol use is associated with increased risk of injury in a wide variety of circumstances, including automobile crashes, falls, and fires ( Cherpitel 1992 ; Freedland et al. 1993 ; Hingson and Howland 1993 ; Hurst et al. 1994 ). Research shows that as people drink increasing quantities of alcohol, their risk of injury increases steadily and the risk begins to rise at relatively low levels of consumption ( Cherpitel et al. 1995 ). An analysis of risk in relation to alcohol use in the hours leading up to an injury has suggested that the amount of alcohol consumed during the 6 hours prior to injury is related directly to the likelihood of injury occurrence ( Vinson et al. 1995 ). The evidence showed a dose-response relationship between intake and injury risk and found no level of drinking to be without risk.

Patterns of alcohol consumption also increase the risk of violence and the likelihood that aggressive behavior will escalate ( Cherpitel 1994 ; Martin 1992 ; Martin and Bachman 1997 ; Norton and Morgan 1989 ; Zhang et al. 1997 ). Alcohol appears to interact with personality characteristics, such as impulsiveness and other factors related to a personal propensity for violence ( Lang 1993 ; Zhang et al. 1997 ). Violence-related trauma also appears to be more closely linked to alcohol dependence symptoms than to other types of alcohol-related injury ( Cherpitel 1997 ).

Patterns of moderate drinking, on the other hand, have been associated with a key health benefit—that is, a lower CHD risk. Research is now in progress to clarify the extent to which alcohol itself, or other factors or surrogates such as lifestyle, diet, exercise, or additives to alcoholic beverages, may be responsible for the lower risk. Broader means of quantifying the relationships between relative risks and specific consumption levels and patterns are needed to describe epidemiologic findings more clearly and simply, and translate them into improved public health strategies.

The Overall Impact

The overall impact of alcohol consumption on mortality can be assessed in two ways ( Rehm and Bondy 1998 ): (1) by conducting meta-analyses using epidemiologic studies that examine all factors contributing to mortality, or (2) by combining risk for various alcohol-caused diseases with a weighted prevalence or incidence of each respective disease.

The meta-analysis approach to assessing overall mortality was used by researchers to examine the results of 16 studies, 10 of which were conducted in the United States ( English et al. 1995 ). In this overview, researchers found the relationship between alcohol intake and mortality for both men and women to be J-shaped curves: the lowest observed risk for overall mortality was associated with an average of 10 grams of alcohol (less than one drink) per day for men and less for women. An average intake of 20 grams (between one and two drinks) per day for women was associated with a significantly increased risk of death compared with abstainers. The risk for women continued to rise with increased consumption and was 50 percent higher among those consuming an average of 40 grams of alcohol (between three and four drinks) per day than among abstainers. Men who averaged 30 grams of alcohol (two drinks) per day had the same mortality as abstainers, whereas a significant increase in mortality was found for those consuming at least 40 grams of alcohol per day.

The proposed J-shaped relationship between alcohol intake and mortality does not apply in all cases, however. For example, because most of the physiologic benefit of moderate drinking is confined to ischemic cardiovascular conditions, such as CHD, in areas of the world where there is little mortality from cardiovascular diseases, alcohol provides little or no reduction in overall mortality. Rather, the relationship between intake and all-cause mortality assumes more of a direct, linear shape ( Murray and Lopez 1996 c ), with increasing consumption associated with higher overall mortality. The same holds true for people under age 45, who have little ischemic cardiovascular mortality ( Andréasson et al. 1988 , 1991; Rehm and Sempos 1995 ).

Quantifying the level of disability and morbidity related to alcohol can be difficult, in large part because few standardized measures exist. One way to quantify the relationship between alcohol and health-related consequences is to use a measure called the disability-adjusted life year (DALY), which may prove useful in summarizing the effects of alcohol on the full spectrum of health outcomes.

In the Global Burden of Disease Study ( Murray and Lopez, 1996 , 1997 b ), the researchers combined years of life lost and years lived with disability into a single indicator, DALY, in which each year lived with a disability was adjusted according to the severity of the disability ( Murray and Lopez 1997 b , c ). The study found tremendous differences in alcohol’s impact on disability across different regions of the world. The most pronounced overall effect was observed in established market economies. The researchers found the smallest effect of alcohol in the Middle Eastern crescent, which is not surprising given the region’s high proportion of abstinent Islamic populations ( Murray and Lopez 1997 a ).

Epidemiologic studies have long provided evidence of the harm alcohol can cause to individual health and to society as a whole. Newer studies have identified an association between low to moderate alcohol consumption and reduced CHD risk and overall mortality. The most significant association with lower CHD risk is largely confined to middle-aged and older individuals in industrialized countries with high rates of cardiovascular diseases. Elucidation of the mechanisms by which alcohol affects CHD risk will clarify the relationship and may enable scientists to develop pharmacologic agents that could mimic or facilitate the positive effect of alcohol on health ( Hennekens 1996 ; UK Inter-Departmental Working Group 1995 ; USDA 1995 ). At this point, research clearly indicates that no pattern of drinking is without risks. However, for individuals who continue to consume alcohol, certain drinking patterns may help reduce these risks considerably.

Among teenagers and young adults in particular, the risks of alcohol use outweigh any benefits that may accrue later in life, since alcohol abuse and dependence and alcohol-related violent behavior and injuries are all too common in young people and are not easily predicted. To determine the likely net outcome of alcohol consumption, the probable risks and benefits for each drinker must be carefully weighed.

  • Andréasson S, Allebeck P, Romelsjo A. Alcohol and mortality among young men: Longitudinal study of Swedish conscripts. British Medical Journal. 1988; 296 (6628):1021–1025. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Baum-Baicker C. The psychological benefits of moderate alcohol consumption: A review of the literature. Drug Alcohol Dependence. 1985; 15 (4):305–322. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Becker U, Deis A, Sorensen TI, Gronbaek M, Borch-Johnsen K, Muller CF, Schnohr P, Jensen G. Prediction of risk of liver disease by alcohol intake, sex, and age: A prospective population study. Hepatology. 1996; 23 (5):1025–1029. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Berger K, Ajani UA, Kase CS, Gaziano JM, Buring JE, Glynn RJ, Hennekens CH. Light-to-moderate alcohol consumption and the risk of stroke among U.S. male physicians. New England Journal of Medicine. 1999; 341 (21):1557–1564. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bode C, Bode JC. Alcohol’s role in gastrointestinal tract disorders. Alcohol Health & Research World. 1997; 21 (1):76–83. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bode JC, Bode C. Alcohol malnutrition and the gastrointestinal tract. In: Watson RR, Watzl B, editors. Nutrition and Alcohol. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; 1992. pp. 403–428. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bruun K. Alcohol Control Policies in Public Health Perspective. Vol. 25. Helsinki, Finland: Finnish Foundation for Alcohol Studies; 1975. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Camargo CA, Jr, Stampfer MJ, Glynn RJ, Gaziano JM, Manson JE, Goldhaber SZ, Hennekens CH. Prospective study of moderate alcohol consumption and risk of peripheral arterial disease in U.S. male physicians. Circulation. 1997; 95 (3):577–580. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Campbell NR, Ashley MJ, Carruthers SG, Lacourciere Y, McKay DW. Lifestyle modifications to prevent and control hypertension. 3. Recommendations on alcohol consumption. Canadian Hypertension Society, Canadian Coalition for High Blood Pressure Prevention and Control, Laboratory Centre for Disease Control at Health Canada, Heart and Stroke Foundation of Canada. Canadian Medical Association Journal. 1999; 160 (suppl 9):S13–S20. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Castaneda R, Cushman P. Alcohol withdrawal: A review of clinical management. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1989; 50 (8):278–284. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chadwick DJ, Goode JA, editors. Alcohol and Cardiovascular Diseases: Novartis Foundation Symposium 216. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1998. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cherpitel CJ. Epidemiology of alcohol-related trauma. Alcohol Health & Research World. 1992; 16 (3):191–196. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cherpitel CJ. Alcohol and injuries resulting from violence: A review of emergency room studies. Addiction. 1994; 89 (2):157–165. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cherpitel CJ. Alcohol and violence-related injuries in the emergency room. In: Galanter M, editor. Recent Developments in Alcoholism. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1997. pp. 105–118. (Volume. 13. Alcohol and Violence: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Psychology, and Family Issues). [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Cherpitel CJ, Tam T, Midanik L, Caetano R, Greenfield T. Alcohol and non-fatal injury in the U.S. general population: A risk function analysis. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 1995; 27 (5):651–661. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Criqui MH. Alcohol and coronary heart disease consistent relationship and public health implications. Clinica Chimica Acta. 1996a; 246 (1–2):51–57. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Criqui MH. Moderate drinking benefits and risks. In: Zakhari S, Wassef M, editors. Alcohol and the Cardiovascular System. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; 1996b. pp. 117–123. (NIAAA Research Monograph No. 31). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dawson DA, Grant BF. Family history of alcoholism and gender: Their combined effects on DSM-IV alcohol dependence and major depression. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1998; 59 (1):97–106. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dawson DA, Grant BF, Chou SP, Pickering RP. Subgroup variation in U.S. drinking patterns: Results of the 1992 National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Study. Journal of Substance Abuse. 1995; 7 :331–344. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Doll R, Forman D, La Vecchia D, Woutersen R. Alcoholic beverages and cancers of the digestive tract and larynx. In: Verschuren PM, editor. Health Issues Related to Alcohol Consumption. Washington, DC: International Life Sciences Institute Press; 1993. pp. 125–166. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Doll R, Peto R, Hall E, Wheatley K, Gray R. Mortality in relation to consumption of alcohol 13 years—observations on male British doctors. British Medical Journal. 1994; 309 (6959):911–918. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dufouil C, Ducimetiere P, Alperovitch A. Sex differences in the association between alcohol consumption and cognitive performance. EVA Study Group. Epidemiology of Vascular Aging. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1997; 146 (5):405–412. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Edwards G, Anderson P, Babor TF, Casswell S, Ferrence R, Giesbrecht N, Godfrey C, Holder HD, Lemmens P, Makela K, Midanik LT, Norstrom T, Osterberg E, Romelsjo A, Room R, Simpura J, Skog O-J. Alcohol Policy and the Public Good. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1994. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • English DR, Holman CDJ, Milne E, Winter MJ, Hulse GK, Codde G, Bower CI, Cortu B, de Klerk N, Lewin GF, Knuiman M, Kurinczuk JJ, Ryan GA. The Quantification of Drug Caused Morbidity and Mortality in Australia, 1992. Canberra, Australia: Canberra Commonwealth Department of Human Services and Health; 1995. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fillmore KM, Golding JM, Graves KL, Kniep S, Leino EV, Romelsjo A, Shoemaker C, Ager CR, Allebeck P, Ferrer HP. Alcohol consumption and mortality. I. Characteristics of drinking groups. Addiction. 1998; 93 (2):183–203. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Freedland ES, McMicken DB, D’Onofrio G. Alcohol and trauma. Emergency Medicine Clinics of North America. 1993; 11 (1):225–239. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fuchs CS, Stampfer MJ, Colditz GA, Giovannucci EL, Manson JE, Kawachi I, Hunter DJ, Hankinson SE, Hennekens CH, Rosner B. Alcohol consumption and mortality among women. New England Journal of Medicine. 1995; 332 (19):1245–1250. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gavaler JS, Arria AM. Increased susceptibility of women to alcoholic liver disease: Artifactual or real? In: Hall PM, editor. Alcoholic Liver Disease: Pathology and Pathogenesis. 2d ed. London, UK: Edward Arnold; 1995. pp. 123–133. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Goldman SA, Brown SA, Christiansen BA. Expectancy theory think about drinking. In: Blane HT, Leonard KE, editors. Psychological Theories of Drinking and Alcoholism. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1987. pp. 181–226. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grant BF. Estimates of U.S. children exposed to alcohol abuse and dependence in the family. American Journal of Public Health. 2000; 90 (1):112–115. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Grant BF, Harford TC, Dawson DA, Chou P, DuFour M, Pickering R. Prevalence of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: United States, 1992. Epidemiologic Bulletin No. 35. Alcohol Health & Research World. 1994; 18 (3):243–248. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Harford TC, Grant BF, Hasin DS. Effect of average daily consumption and frequency of intoxication on the occurrence of dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems. In: Clark WB, Hilton ME, editors. Alcohol in America: Drinking Practices and Problems. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press; 1991. pp. 212–237. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hauge R, Irgens-Jensen O. The experiencing of positive consequences of drinking in four Scandinavian countries. British Journal of Addiction. 1990; 85 (5):645–653. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Heath DB. A decade of development in the anthropological study of alcohol use: 1970–1980. In: Douglas M, editor. Constructive Drinking: Perspectives on Drink From Anthropology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1987. pp. 16–69. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hennekens C. Alcohol and risk of coronary events. In: Zakhari S, Wassef M, editors. Alcohol and the Cardiovascular System. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; 1996. pp. 15–24. (NIAAA Research Monograph 1996 No. 31). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hillbom M, Juvela S. Alcohol and risk for stroke. In: Zakhari S, Wassef M, editors. Alcohol and the Cardiovascular System. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; 1996. pp. 63–83. (NIAAA Research Monograph No. 31). [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hillbom M, Haapaniemi H, Juvela S, Palomaki H, Numminen H, Kaste M. Recent alcohol consumption, cigarette smoking, and cerebral infarction in young adults. Stroke. 1995; 26 (1):40–45. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hilton ME. Demographic distribution of drinking problems in 1984. In: Clark WB, Hilton ME, editors. Alcohol in America: Drinking Practices and Problems. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press; 1991a. pp. 87–101. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hilton ME. Note on measuring drinking problems in the 1984 National Alcohol Survey. In: Clark WB, Hilton ME, editors. Alcohol in America: Drinking Practices and Problems. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press; 1991b. pp. 51–70. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hingson R, Howland J. Alcohol and non-traffic unintended injuries. Addiction. 1993; 88 (7):877–883. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hisatomi S, Kumashiro R, Sata M, Ishii K, Tanikawa K. Gender difference in alcoholic and liver disease in Japan: An analysis based on histological findings. Hepatology Research. 1997; 8 (2):113–120. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hurst PM, Harte D, Firth WJ. The Grand Rapids dip revisited. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 1994; 26 (5):647–654. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • International Agency for Research on Cancer. Alcohol Drinking. Lyon, France: International Agency for Research on Cancer; 1988. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jepson RG, Fowkes FG, Donnan PT, Housley E. Alcohol intake as a risk factor for peripheral arterial disease in the general population in the Edinburgh Artery Study. European Journal of Epidemiology. 1995; 11 (1):9–14. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kannel WB, McGee DL. Update on some epidemiologic features of intermittent claudication: The Framingham Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 1985; 33 (1):13–18. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Keil U, Chambless LE, Döring A, Filipiak B, Stieber J. The relation of alcohol intake to coronary heart disease and all-cause mortality in a beer-drinking population. Epidemiology. 1997; 8 (2):150–156. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kessler RC, Nelson CB, McGonagle KA, Edlund MJ, Frank RG, Leaf PJ. The epidemiology of co-occurring addictive and mental disorders: Implications for prevention and service utilization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1996; 66 (1):17–31. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kessler RC, Crum RM, Warner LA, Nelson CB, Schulenberg J, Anthony JC. Lifetime co-occurrence of DSM-III-R alcohol abuse and dependence with other psychiatric disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1997; 54 (4):313–321. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Kitamura A, Iso H, Sankai T, Naito Y, Sato S, Kiyama M, Okamura T, Nakagawa Y, Iida M, Shimamoto T, Komachi Y. Alcohol intake and premature coronary heart disease in urban Japanese men. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1998; 147 (1):59–65. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lang A. Alcohol-related violence an individual offender focus. In: Martin SE, editor. Alcohol and Interpersonal Violence: Fostering Multidisciplinary Perspectives. Rockville, MD: 1993. pp. 221–236. (NIAAA Research Monograph No. 24). NIH Pub. No. 93–3496. [ Google Scholar ]
  • La Vecchia C, Negri E. The role of alcohol in oesophageal cancer in non-smokers, and of tobacco in non-drinkers. International Journal of Cancer. 1989; 43 (5):784–785. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leigh BC. In search of the Seven Dwarves: Issues of measurement and meaning in alcohol expectancy research. Psychological Bulletin. 1989; 105 (3):361–373. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Leigh BC, Stacy AW. On the scope of alcohol expectancy research: Remaining issues of measurement and meaning. Psychological Bulletin. 1991; 110 (1):147–154. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mäkelä K, Mustonen H. Positive and negative experiences related to drinking as a function of annual alcohol intake. British Journal of Addiction. 1988; 83 (4):403–408. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mäkelä K, Simpura J. Experiences related to drinking as a function of annual alcohol intake and by sex and age. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1985; 15 (4):389–404. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martin SE. Epidemiology of alcohol-related interpersonal violence. Alcohol Health & Research World. 1992; 16 (3):230–237. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Martin SE, Bachman R. The relationship of alcohol to injury in assault cases. In: Galanter M, editor. Recent Developments in Alcoholism. New York: Plenum Press; 1997. pp. 41–56. (Vol. 13. Alcoholism and Violence: Epidemiology, Neurobiology, Psychology, and Family Issues). [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mingardi R, Avogaro A, Noventa F, Strazzabosco M, Stocchiero C, Tiengo A, Erle G. Alcohol intake is associated with a lower prevalence of peripheral vascular disease in non-insulin dependent diabetic women. Nutrition Metabolism and Cardiovascular Disease. 1997; 7 (4):301–308. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Murray CJL, Lopez AD. The Global Burden of Disease: A Comprehensive Assessment of Mortality and Disability From Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors in 1990 and Projected to 2020. Cambridge, MA: Harvard School of Public Health; 1996. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Murray CJ, Lopez AD. Global mortality, disability, and the contribution of risk factors: Global Burden of Disease Study. Lancet. 1997a; 349 (9063):1436–1442. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Murray CJ, Lopez AD. Mortality by cause for eight regions of the world: Global Burden of Disease Study. Lancet. 1997b; 349 (9061):1269–1276. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Murray CJ, Lopez AD. Regional patterns of disability-free life expectancy and disability-adjusted life expectancy: Global Burden of Disease Study. Lancet. 1997c; 349 (9062):1347–1352. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Naveau S, Giraud V, Borotto E, Aubert A, Capron F, Chaput JC. Excess weight risk factor for alcoholic liver disease. Hepatology. 1997; 25 (1):108–111. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Norton RN, Morgan MY. The role of alcohol in mortality and morbidity from inter-personal violence. Alcohol. 1989; 24 (6):565–576. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Orgogozo JM, Dartigues JF, Lafont S, Letenneur L, Commenges D, Salamon R, Renaud S, Breteler MB. Wine consumption and dementia in the elderly: A prospective community study from the Bordeaux area. Revue Neurologique (Paris) 1997; 153 (3):185–192. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rehm J, Bondy S. Alcohol and all-cause mortality: An overview. In: Chadwick DJ, Goode JA, editors. Alcohol and Cardiovascular Diseases: Novartis Foundation Symposium 216. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1998. pp. 223–236. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rehm J, Sempos CT. Alcohol consumption and all-cause mortality. Addiction. 1995; 90 (4):471–480. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rehm JT, Bondy SJ, Sempos CT, Vuong CV. Alcohol consumption and coronary heart disease morbidity and mortality. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1997; 146 (6):495–501. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Seitz H, Pöschl G. Alcohol and gastrointestinal cancer: Pathogenic mechanisms. Addiction Biology. 1997; 2 (1):19–33. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Thun MJ, Peto R, Lopez AD, Monaco JH, Henley SJ, Heath CW, Doll R. Alcohol consumption and mortality among middle-aged and elderly U.S. adults. New England Journal of Medicine. 1997; 337 (24):1705–1714. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tyas SL. Are tobacco and alcohol use related to Alzheimer’s disease? A critical assessment of the evidence and its implications. Addiction Biology. 1996; 1 (3):237–254. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • You RX, McNeil JJ, O’Malley HM, Davis SM, Thrift AG, Donnan GA. Risk factors for stroke due to cerebral infarction in young adults. Stroke. 1997; 28 (10):1913–1918. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • UK Inter-Departmental Working Group. Report on Sensible Drinking. London, UK: Department of Health; 1995. [ Google Scholar ]
  • U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy People 2000. Midcourse Review and 1995 Revisions. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, U.S. Public Health Service; 1995. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Vinson DC, Mabe N, Leonard LL, Alexander J, Becker J, Boyer J, Moll J. Alcohol and injury. A case-crossover study. Archives of Family Medicine. 1995; 4 (6):505–511. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • World Cancer Research Fund and American Institute for Cancer Research. Food, Nutrition and the Prevention of Cancer: A Global Perspective. Washington, DC: American Institute for Cancer Research; 1997. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zakhari S. Alcohol and the cardiovascular system: Molecular mechanisms for beneficial and harmful action. Alcohol Health & Research World. 1997; 21 (1):21–29. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zhang L, Wieczorek WF, Welte JW. The nexus between alcohol and violent crime. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1997; 21 (7):1264–1271. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]

IMAGES

  1. Alcoholism: Symptoms and Treatment Research Paper Example

    alcoholism research paper

  2. Research paper on alcoholism pdf

    alcoholism research paper

  3. Research Paper About Alcohol

    alcoholism research paper

  4. 💄 Alcoholism research paper. Concept and Treatment of Alcohol Abuse

    alcoholism research paper

  5. 😍 Research paper about alcohol. Research Paper On Alcohol. 2019-01-27

    alcoholism research paper

  6. 💄 Alcoholism research paper. Concept and Treatment of Alcohol Abuse

    alcoholism research paper